Artigo Revisado por pares

Driving toward a goal and the goal‐gradient hypothesis: the impact of goal proximity on compliance rate, donation size, and fatigue

2013; Wiley; Volume: 43; Issue: 9 Linguagem: Inglês

10.1111/jasp.12152

ISSN

1559-1816

Autores

Jakob D. Jensen, Andy J. King, Nick Carcioppolo,

Tópico(s)

Death Anxiety and Social Exclusion

Resumo

Journal of Applied Social PsychologyVolume 43, Issue 9 p. 1881-1895 Original Article Driving toward a goal and the goal-gradient hypothesis: the impact of goal proximity on compliance rate, donation size, and fatigue Jakob D. Jensen, Corresponding Author Jakob D. Jensen Department of Communication and Department of Health Promotion and Education, University of Utah Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jakob D. Jensen, Department of Communication, University of Utah, 2423 LNCO, 255 S. Central Campus Drive, Salt Lake City, UT 84112. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorAndy J. King, Andy J. King College of Media and Communication, Texas Tech UniversitySearch for more papers by this authorNick Carcioppolo, Nick Carcioppolo Department of Communication Studies, University of MiamiSearch for more papers by this author Jakob D. Jensen, Corresponding Author Jakob D. Jensen Department of Communication and Department of Health Promotion and Education, University of Utah Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jakob D. Jensen, Department of Communication, University of Utah, 2423 LNCO, 255 S. Central Campus Drive, Salt Lake City, UT 84112. E-mail: [email protected]Search for more papers by this authorAndy J. King, Andy J. King College of Media and Communication, Texas Tech UniversitySearch for more papers by this authorNick Carcioppolo, Nick Carcioppolo Department of Communication Studies, University of MiamiSearch for more papers by this author First published: 21 August 2013 https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12152Citations: 19Read the full textAboutPDF ToolsRequest permissionExport citationAdd to favoritesTrack citation ShareShare Give accessShare full text accessShare full-text accessPlease review our Terms and Conditions of Use and check box below to share full-text version of article.I have read and accept the Wiley Online Library Terms and Conditions of UseShareable LinkUse the link below to share a full-text version of this article with your friends and colleagues. Learn more.Copy URL Share a linkShare onEmailFacebookTwitterLinkedInRedditWechat Abstract Driving toward a goal (DTAG) is a compliance technique derived from observed persuasion practice (e.g., telethons) wherein the persuader utilizes a goal pitch (e.g., "Help us raise $500") and progress toward a goal (e.g., a tote board) to encourage compliance. It was postulated that DTAG would be more effective than legitimizing a paltry contribution (LPC) at increasing compliance rate, size, and stability. In Study 1, a fundraising field experiment (N = 840 donations) found that LPC garnered significantly more donations and DTAG garnered significantly larger donations. In Study 2, a lab experiment (N = 992 participants) found that LPC garnered more donations at Time 1, DTAG garnered more donations over time (eventually matching LPC), and LPC yielded smaller donations over time. References Andrews, K. R., Carpenter, C. J., Shaw, A. S., & Boster, F. J. (2008). The legitimization of paltry favors effect: A review and meta-analysis. Communication Reports, 21, 59–69. Barnes, M. (2006). Reducing donor fatigue syndrome. Nonprofit World, 24(2), 1–8. Brockner, J., Guzzi, B., Kane, J., Levine, E., & Shaplen, K. (1984). Organizational fundraising: Further evidence on the effects of legitimizing small donations. Journal of Consumer Research, 11, 611–614. Brownstein, R. J., & Katzev, R. D. (1985). The relative effectiveness of three compliance techniques in eliciting donations to a cultural organization. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 564–574. Burger, J. M. (1986). Increasing compliance by improving the deal: The that's-not-all technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 277–283. Cialdini, R. B. (2009). Influence: Science and practice ( 5th ed.). Boston: Pearson. Cialdini, R. B., Cacioppo, J. T., Bassett, R., & Miller, J. A. (1978). The low-ball procedure for producing compliance: Commitment then cost. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 463–476. Cialdini, R. B., & Schroeder, D. A. (1976). Increasing compliance by legitimizing paltry contributions: When even a penny helps. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 599–604. Cialdini, R. B., Vincent, J. E., Lewis, S. K., Catalan, J., Wheeler, D., & Darby, B. L. (1975). Reciprocal concessions procedure for inducing compliance: The door-in-the-face technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 599–605. Cook, T. D., & Flay, B. R. (1978). The persistence of experimentally induced attitude change. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 11, pp. 1–57). New York: Academic Press. DeJong, W., & Oopik, A. J. (1992). Effect of legitimizing small contributions and labeling potential donors as "helpers" on responses to a direct mail solicitation for charity. Psychological Reports, 71, 923–928. Dolinski, D., Grzyb, T., Olejnik, J., Prusakowski, S., & Urban, K. (2005). Let's dialogue about penny: Effectiveness of dialogue involvement and legitimizing paltry contribution techniques. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 35, 1150–1170. Fraser, C., Hite, R. E., & Sauer, P. L. (1988). Increasing contributions in solicitation campaigns: The use of large and small anchorpoints. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 284–287. Freedman, J., & Fraser, S. (1966). Compliance without pressure: The foot-in-the-door technique. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 195–202. Glynn, S. A., Busch, M. P., Schreiber, G. B., Murphy, E. L., Wright, D. J., Tu, Y., et al. (2003). Effect of a national disaster on blood supply and safety: The September 11 experience. Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 2246–2253. Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs, 76, 408–420. Heilizer, F. (1977). A review of theory and research on the assumptions of Miller's response competitions models: Response gradients. Journal of General Psychology, 97, 17–71. Hull, C. L. (1932). The goal-gradient hypothesis and maze learning. Psychological Review, 39, 25–43. Hull, C. L. (1934). The rats' speed of locomotion gradient in approach to food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 17, 393–422. Jensen, J. D., Carcioppolo, N., King, A. J., Bernat, J. K., Davis, L. A., Yale, R., et al. (2011). Including limitations in news coverage of cancer research: Effects of news hedging on fatalism, medical skepticism, patient trust, and backlash. Journal of Health Communication, 16, 486–503. Joule, R. V., Gouilloux, F., & Weber, F. (2001). The lure: A new compliance procedure. Journal of Social Psychology, 129, 741–749. Kam, C. D., & Ramos, J. M. (2008). Joining and leaving the rally: Understanding the surge and decline in Presidential approval following 9/11. Public Opinion Quarterly, 72, 619–650. Kinnick, K., Krugman, D. M., & Cameron, G. T. (1996). Compassion fatigue: Communication and burnout toward social problems. Journalism and Mass Communication Quarterly, 73, 687–707. Kivetz, R., Urminsky, O., & Zheng, Y. (2006). The goal-gradient hypothesis resurrected: Purchase acceleration, illusionary goal progress, and customer retention. Journal of Marketing Research, 43, 39–58. Lapinski, M. K., & Rimal, R. N. (2005). An explication of social norms. Communication Theory, 15, 127–147. Lock, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Mark, M. M., & Shotland, R. L. (1983). Increasing charitable contributions: An experimental evaluation of the American Cancer Society's recommended solicitation procedures. Journal of Voluntary Action Research, 12, 8–21. Mueller, J. E. (1970). Presidential popularity from Truman to Johnson. American Political Science Review, 64, 18–34. Muscular Dystrophy Association. (2011). Jerry Lewis MDA Labor Day telethon. Retrieved February 9, 2011, from http://www.mdausa.org/telethon/ O'Brien, R. G., & Kaiser, M. K. (1985). The MANOVA approach for analyzing repeated measures designs: An extensive primer. Psychological Bulletin, 97, 316–333. O'Keefe, D. J., & Jensen, J. D. (2006). The advantages of compliance or the disadvantages of noncompliance? A meta-analytic review of the relative persuasive effectiveness of gain-framed and loss-framed messages. Communication Yearbook, 30, 1–43. O'Neal, J. R., & Bryan, A. L. (1995). The rally 'round the flag effect in U.S. foreign policy crises: 1950–1985. Political Behavior, 17, 379–401. Perrine, R. M., & Heather, S. (2000). Effects of picture and even-a-penny-will-help appeals on anonymous donations to charity. Psychological Reports, 86, 551–559. Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. Reeves, R. A., Macolini, R. M., & Martin, R. C. (1987). Legitimizing paltry contributions: On-the-spot vs. mail-in requests. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 731–738. Reeves, R. A., & Saucer, P. R. (1993). A test of commitment in legitimizing paltry contributions. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 8, 537–544. Reingen, P. H. (1978). On inducing compliance with requests. Journal of Consumer Research, 5, 96–102. Rhoades, K., & Cialdini, R. (2002). The business of influence: Principles that lead to success in commercial settings. In J. P. Dillard & M. Pfau (Eds.), The persuasion handbook: Developments in theory and practice (pp. 513–542). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Rimal, R. N. (2005). How behaviors are influenced by perceived norms: A test of the theory of normative social behavior. Communication Research, 32, 389–414. Rimal, R. N., & Real, K. (2003). Understanding the influence of perceived norms on behaviors. Communication Theory, 13, 184–203. Weyant, J. M. (1984). Applying social psychology to induce charitable donations. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 14, 441–447. Weyant, J. M., & Smith, S. L. (1987). Getting more by asking for less: The effects of request size on donations of charity. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 17, 392–400. Citing Literature Volume43, Issue9September 2013Pages 1881-1895 ReferencesRelatedInformation

Referência(s)