Artigo Acesso aberto Revisado por pares

Mirror neuron research: the past and the future

2014; Royal Society; Volume: 369; Issue: 1644 Linguagem: Inglês

10.1098/rstb.2013.0169

ISSN

1471-2970

Autores

Pier Francesco Ferrari, Giacomo Rizzolatti,

Tópico(s)

Virtual Reality Applications and Impacts

Resumo

You have accessMoreSectionsView PDF ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinked InRedditEmail Cite this article Ferrari Pier Francesco and Rizzolatti Giacomo 2014Mirror neuron research: the past and the futurePhil. Trans. R. Soc. B3692013016920130169http://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0169SectionYou have accessIntroductionMirror neuron research: the past and the future Pier Francesco Ferrari Pier Francesco Ferrari Dipartimento di Neuroscience, Università di Parma, 43125 Parma, Italy [email protected] Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author and Giacomo Rizzolatti Giacomo Rizzolatti Dipartimento di Neuroscience, Università di Parma, 43125 Parma, Italy Centre for Brain and Motor Cognition, Italian Institute of Technology, Via Volturno 39, Parma, Italy Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author Pier Francesco Ferrari Pier Francesco Ferrari Dipartimento di Neuroscience, Università di Parma, 43125 Parma, Italy [email protected] Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed and Giacomo Rizzolatti Giacomo Rizzolatti Dipartimento di Neuroscience, Università di Parma, 43125 Parma, Italy Centre for Brain and Motor Cognition, Italian Institute of Technology, Via Volturno 39, Parma, Italy Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Published:05 June 2014https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0169This special issue is, in large part, based on the presentations given at a workshop held at the 'Ettore Majorana' Centre in Erice (Sicily) to celebrate the discovery of mirror neurons, 20 years after the first report on their discovery [1]. All articles in this issue have been updated and deal with the present state of the art of mirror research in different fields.It was certainly difficult to predict from the note published in 1992 how profoundly the discovery of mirror neurons would influence cognitive neuroscience in the next 20 years. Even more difficult was to forecast that the discovery of mirror neurons would have an impact on disciplines outside neurosciences, such as psychology, ethology, sociology and philosophy, or that they would interest novelists (e.g. The elegance of the hedgehog [2]) and laymen.Why has the notion of mirror neurons had such a wide impact on cognitive disciplines? Two reasons are the most likely. The first is that their discovery put the problem of how we understand others at the forefront of neuroscience. The second is that, by showing that mirror neurons were basically motor neurons, they suggested a rather unexpected solution to this problem: the motor system is involved in understanding the actions and intentions of others.This problem of how we understand others is not new in philosophical debates. One of the most accepted views is that the capacity to understand the intentions of others is based on the observer's capacity to infer others' internal mental states and to ascribe to them a causal role in generating the observed behaviour [3,4]. This view is in line with the analytical tradition based on the propositional account of others' minds. An alternative view, put forward by phenomenologists, is that we understand others by comparing an action done by others with our own behaviour in a similar situation [5]. The discovery of mirror neurons did not disprove the conventional analytical view, but demonstrated the validity of the phenomenological stance, at least in most everyday life conditions. It is important to stress that action understanding through the mirror mechanism is a direct activation of motor representation. It does not require a cognitive simulation of others' behaviour as suggested by simulation theory of action understanding [6].Interest in the direct understanding of others' actions increased enormously when it was found that the mirror mechanism is also present in the emotional brain centres. Although there is no doubt that one can understand others' emotions via inferential mental processes (as during the observation of emotions), there is clear evidence that brain structures involved in the integration and control of emotions, like the insula and the anterior cingulate, respond both when one feels an emotion (e.g. pain or disgust) owing to natural stimuli, or when one observes that emotion in others [7–10]. This mechanism allows a direct first-person understanding of others' emotion. 'Your pain is my pain'.Some authors previously questioned the presence of mirror neurons in humans [11]. This is not, however, a current issue, given the overwhelming evidence for the existence of mirror neurons in humans from hundreds of experiments [12] carried out with a variety of techniques (positron emission tomography, functional magnetic resonance imaging, transcranial magnetic stimulation, magnetoencephalography and electroencephalography), as well as from a few single neuron studies [13]. It is, nevertheless, of some interest to examine the reasons why this somehow surprising debate took place. In fact, traditionally, the mechanisms discovered in primates as well as in other species of animals are considered, at least prima facie, to exist also in humans. Nobody questioned, for example, the existence of 'simple' and 'complex' neurons in the visual cortex of humans, despite the fact that they were first discovered in the cat and then studied in detail in the monkey [14,15].One reason for the opposition of some neuropsychologists to accept the existence of mirror neurons in humans is probably due to the fact that the mirror mechanism was discovered when it was not completely clear whether damage to the areas endowed with this mechanism determines deficits in understanding others' actions (for evidence for the causative role of mirror neurons in action understanding, see [12,16]). Typically, the opposite series of events occurs, that is experimental and/or clinical studies indicate that a certain cortical region is involved in a particular function (e.g. occipital cortex in vision) and then single neuron recordings in animals reveal the mechanism underlying this function.The second reason is that the motor system has been traditionally considered not to be involved in cognitive functions [17]. Its role was thought to be limited to the control and production of movements. The discovery of mirror neurons as well as of other motor neurons involved in cognition, such as neurons coding peripersonal space or neurons transforming object affordances into potential motor acts (canonical neurons), radically changed this view. Yet, as often occurs in science, there was some resistance to changing consolidated points of view.An exciting and not fully explored field is the origin of mirror neurons and how the development of social-cognitive skills in infancy may rely on the maturation of the motor system. We gave ample space in this special issue to this topic [18–22]. It is highly debated whether the mirror system arose as the consequence of association learning or an evolutionary process that endowed a population of neurons with a mechanism necessary for accomplishing a specific function [20,23].According to the associative account, mirror neurons emerge during development as the result of strengthening sensorimotor connections that originally evolved for visuomotor control [20]. From this perspective, for example, the motor command for reaching/grasping made by the infant is associated with the sight of his/her own hand. The simultaneous firing of motor and visual neurons can strengthen their connections (see also [24,25]). The further step would be to generalize the visual description of the agent's action to that of others and therefore the observation of others' actions is sufficient to trigger the observer's motor neurons.The alternative account maintains that at least part of the mirror neuron system is genetically determined and adapted to serve one function as the consequence of the natural selection process ([20], but also see [26,27]). More recently, models have been proposed which avoid pitting one view against the other, thus escaping an old fruitless nature–nurture divide. Rather, these models integrate learning processes into a more realistic theoretical framework on how the brain develops in the peri- and post-natal period, and on how some brain mechanisms do not need to be specified by strict genetic rules. It is known that plastic changes do occur in the brain during ontogeny and involve a complex interaction between the environment and gene expression in different brain networks. Nobody could deny, for example, that the development of vision and the organization of the visual cortex in primates and cats, and of human language and song learning in birds, are examples of adaptations that, in order to develop, require genetic instructions as well as specific environmental inputs.One should also consider that neural circuits very rarely evolved for one purpose only. They are often co-opted for other purposes as well; that is, they are exapted to serve additional functions. Thus, the brain networks that are involved in sensorimotor transformation can be recruited to support additional functions such as action understanding and imitation.The key role of epigenetic events in driving neuronal development has recently been emphasized [26]. These epigenetic events might facilitate and stabilize, in the course of phylogeny, sensorimotor mechanisms involved in the coding of others' actions (i.e. mirror neurons). The markers of this gene expression can consistently emerge in each generation, when infants face specific and predictable environmental conditions [28], thus leading to long-lasting changes in the phenotype. In fact, in all individuals of our species, infants experience the vision of their own hand and benefit from universal forms of face-to-face interactions with the caregiver. These behavioural events affect those brain mechanisms (including the mirror mechanism) that have been referred as to experience-expectant, which actively 'seek out' specific environmental inputs. If the elements of this 'expectable' environment are either lacking, or else markedly disturbed, then the immature nervous system is likely to be profoundly affected, thus requiring alternative strategies for rewiring the networks. According to the epigenetic hypothesis, new emergent neuronal responses and the related epigenetic mechanisms yielded some advantages for the organism (e.g. faster or more accurate capacity to recognize others' actions through mapping the actions onto one's own motor knowledge), and natural selection would have favoured their stabilization and facilitation of expression [26].An important field for future research studies will be to assess the sensitivity of mirror neurons to early perturbations of the social environment and whether there are critical periods necessary for the wiring of the sensorimotor connections responsible for mirror neuron construction and functioning. Clinical investigations of disturbances of early social interactions, such as in children who experience a severe lack of early social stimulation or who, for genetic reasons, have deficits in social communication, could represent an interesting challenge to explore the ontogeny of this mechanism and its functional role.Another important issue that will be fundamental for understanding the mirror mechanism more deeply is to elucidate its neurochemical and molecular bases. This appears very difficult to address with macaques due to the limitation in applying biomolecular techniques in a species with low reproductive efficiency (one infant per year, long gestation period and delivery interval) and which requires high costs in terms of housing and experiments performing. By contrast, other animal models, such as marmosets or birds, appear to be suitable for investigating complex social-cognitive skills and their neural underpinnings.Marmosets, for example, have been used with success in biomedical research, not only because of the reduced costs involved, but also because of the possibility of using them in neuroscience research both with invasive and non-invasive techniques (to use them in neuroscience research both with invasive and non-invasive techniques [29–31]). The results of these and other studies clearly showed that marmosets display remarkable similarities with humans in terms of brain networks, immune response and endocrinological profile. The recent studies on transgenic techniques [31] and on the embryonic stem cells of marmosets indicate that this species may be a more suitable model for genome research and biomolecular investigation than the macaque. These new areas of research, together with the recent discovery of mirror neurons in the ventral premotor cortex of marmosets [32], represent therefore a first critical step in paving the way to new investigations aimed at clarifying the molecular basis and the evolutionary origins of this mechanism in primates.Studies in songbirds are another example of how mirror neuron research has expanded and stimulated the field. Given the complex interactions occurring during development between parents and their offspring which are so critical for song learning, it is not surprising that in songbirds, neurons with mirror properties (audio–motor mirroring) have been found. The work by Mooney and co-workers ([33], see also [34]) clearly demonstrates that, in songbirds, areas involved in song production and learning (i.e. area HVC) do have neurons with mirror properties for audio–motor matching. Lesions in the HVC impair song production and song recognition, as well as the capacity to learn new contingencies. These authors suggest that audio–motor mirror neurons in songbirds may be involved in song learning and communication [34], thus making a striking parallel between the functional role of humans' Broca area in speech production and perception [35] and the HVC in birds. The possibility of investigating the molecular basis of song production and learning in different species of birds makes this model extremely promising in investigating the development and functional role of the mirror mechanism and its molecular basis.Finally, this new information should be extremely useful not only for a better comprehension of the mirror mechanism itself, but also for its practical application in psychiatric and neurological disorders. There is indeed evidence, although still preliminary, of possible underdevelopment or impairment of the mirror system in autism [12] and in those psychiatric disorders [36] in which social competence is compromised [12]. A better knowledge of the molecular and neurochemical basis of this mechanism will represent a breakthrough for establishing new therapies and interventions based on solid experimental findings.AcknowledgementsWe thank the 'Ettore Majorana' Centre in Erice for supporting the workshop on mirror neurons, which stimulated lively scientific exchanges among scholars and young researchers, and that inspired the realization of this special issue. We also thank Lynne Murray for her comments on an early draft of this manuscript.Funding statementThis manuscript was supported by NICHDP01HD064653 grant to P.F.F.FootnotesOne contribution of 19 to a Theme Issue 'Mirror neurons: fundamental discoveries, theoretical perspectives and clinical implications'.© 2014 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.References1di Pellegrino G, Fadiga L, Fogassi L, Gallese V& Rizzolatti G. 1992Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study.Exp. Brain Res. 91, 176–180. (doi:10.1007/BF00230027). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar2Barbery M. 2008The elegance of the hedgehog. London, UK: Gallic Books. Google Scholar3Carruthers P& Smith PK. 1996Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Google Scholar4Malle BF, Moses JL& Baldwin DA. 2001Intentions and intentionality: foundations of social cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar5Merleau-Ponty M. 1945Phénoménologie de la perception. Paris, France: Éditions Gallimard. Google Scholar6Goldman A& de Vignemont F. 2009Is social cognition embodied?Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 154–159. (doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.007). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar7Singer T, Seymour J, O'Doherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ& Frith CD. 2004Empathy for pain involves the affective but not the sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162. (doi:10.1126/science.1093535). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar8Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet JP, Gallese V& Rizzolatti G. 2003Both of us disgusted in my insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664. (doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar9Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau MC, Mazziotta JC& Lenzi GL. 2003Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 100, 5497–5502. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0935845100). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar10Gallese V, Keysers C& Rizzolatti G. 2004A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 396–403. (doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.07.002). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar11Lingnau A, Gesierich B& Caramazza A. 2009Asymmetric fMRI adaptation reveals no evidence for mirror neurons in humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 106, 9925–9930. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0902262106). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar12Rizzolatti G& Fogassi L. 2014The mirror mechanism: recent findings and perspectives. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20130420. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0420). Link, ISI, Google Scholar13Mukamel R, Ekstrom AD, Kaplan J, Iacoboni M& Fried I. 2010Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Curr. Biol. 20, 750–756. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar14Wiesel TN& Hubel DH. 1963Single-cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. J. Neurophysiol. 26, 1003–1017. Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar15Wiesel TN. 1982The postnatal development of the visual cortex and the influence of environment. Biosci. Rep. 2, 351–377. (doi:10.1007/BF01119299). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar16Michael J, Sandberg K, Skewes J, Wolf T, Blicher J, Overgaard M& Frith CD. In press. Continuous theta-burst stimulation demonstrates a causal role of premotor homunculus in action understanding. Psychol. Sci. (doi:10.1177/0956797613520608). ISI, Google Scholar17Rizzolatti G& Sinigaglia C. 2008Further reflections on how we interpret the actions of others. Nature 455, 589. (doi:10.1038/455589b). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar18Hunnius S& Bekkering H. 2014What are you doing? How active and observational experience shape infants' action understanding. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20130490. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0490). Link, ISI, Google Scholar19Simpson EA, Murray L, Paukner A& Ferrari PF. 2014The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20130289. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0289). Link, ISI, Google Scholar20Heyes C. 2014Tinbergen on mirror neurons. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20130180. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0180). Link, ISI, Google Scholar21Marshall PJ& Meltzoff AN. 2014Neural mirroring mechanisms and imitation in human infants. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20130620. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0620). Link, ISI, Google Scholar22Woodward AL& Gerson SA. 2014Mirroring and the development of action understanding. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20130181. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0181). Link, ISI, Google Scholar23Catmur C. 2013Sensorimotor learning and the ontogeny of the mirror neuron system. Neurosci. Lett. 540, 21–27. (doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.10.001). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar24Del Giudice M, Manera V& Keysers C. 2009Programmed to learn? The ontogeny of mirror neurons. Dev. Sci. 12, 350–363. (doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00783.x). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar25Casile A, Caggiano V& Ferrari PF. 2011The mirror neuron system: a fresh view. Neuroscientist 17, 524–538. (doi:10.1177/1073858410392239). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar26Ferrari PF, Tramacere A, Simpson EA& Iriki A. 2013Mirror neurons development through the lens of epigenetics. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 450–457. (doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.07.003). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar27Simpson EA, Murray L, Paukner A& Ferrari PF. 2014The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20130289. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0289). Link, ISI, Google Scholar28Fagiolini M, Jensen CL& Champagne FA. 2009Epigenetic influences on brain development and plasticity. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 207–212. (doi:10.1016/j.conb.2009.05.009). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar29Hikishima K, et al.2011Population-averaged standard template brain atlas for the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). Neuroimage 54, 2741–2749. (doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.061). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar30Okano H, Hikishima K, Iriki A& Sasaki E. 2012The common marmoset as a novel animal model system for biomedical and neuroscience research applications. Semin. Fetal Neonatal Med. 17, 336–340. (doi:10.1016/j.siny.2012.07.002). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar31Sasaki E, et al.2009Generation of transgenic non-human primates with germ line transmission. Nature 459, 523–527. (doi:10.1038/nature08090). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar32Suzuki W, Banno T, Miyakawa N, Abe H& Ichinohe N. 2013Encoding others' action by temporal-frontal circuit including mirror system in marmoset. Soc. Neurosci. 723, 14. Google Scholar33Prather JF, Peters S, Nowicki S& Mooney R. 2008Precise auditory-vocal mirroring in neurons for learned vocal communication. Nature 451, 305–310. (doi:10.1038/nature06492). Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar34Mooney R. 2014Auditory–vocal mirroring in songbirds. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20130179. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0179). Link, ISI, Google Scholar35D'Ausilio A, Maffongelli L, Bartoli E, Campanella M, Ferrari E, Berry J& Fadiga L. 2014Listening to speech recruits specific tongue motor synergies as revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation and tissue-Doppler ultrasound imaging. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 369, 20130418. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2013.0418). Link, ISI, Google Scholar36Mehta UM, Thirthalli J, Basavaraju R, Gangadhar BN& Pascual-Leone A. In press.Reduced mirror neuron activity in schizophrenia and its association with theory of mind deficits: evidence from a transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Schizophr. Bull. (doi:10.1093/schbul/sbt155). ISI, Google Scholar Next Article VIEW FULL TEXT DOWNLOAD PDF FiguresRelatedReferencesDetailsCited byKim M, Choi J and Baek S (2021) Win-Stay-Lose-Shift as a self-confirming equilibrium in the iterated Prisoner's Dilemma, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 288:1953, Online publication date: 30-Jun-2021. De Stefani E, Barbot A, Bertolini C, Belluardo M, Garofalo G, Bruno N, Bianchi B, Ferri A, Ferrari P and Ward N (2021) A New Neurorehabilitative Postsurgery Intervention for Facial Palsy Based on Smile Observation and Hand-Mouth Motor Synergies, Neural Plasticity, 10.1155/2021/8890541, 2021, (1-13), Online publication date: 24-Mar-2021. Lauro Grotto R (2020) Symmetrization, Mirroring and External Reality: An 'Inner' Perspective, European Review, 10.1017/S1062798720000459, 29:2, (181-196), Online publication date: 1-Apr-2021. (2021) Literatur Traum und Traumdeutung in den christlichen Apokryphen, 10.13109/9783666517068.364, (364-404), Online publication date: 12-Jul-2021. Babushkin V, Jamil M, Park W and Eid M Sensorimotor Skill Communication: A Literature Review, IEEE Access, 10.1109/ACCESS.2021.3081449, 9, (75132-75149) Fei D (2021) In what sense should we talk about the perception of other minds?, Cultures of Science, 10.1177/20966083211006489, 3:4, (267-279), Online publication date: 1-Dec-2020. Restivo S (2020) The Social Brain Paradigm Einstein's Brain, 10.1007/978-3-030-32918-1_5, (101-121), . Bird J and Karageorghis C (2020) A Grounded Theory of Music-Video Use in an Exercise Facility, Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 10.1080/02701367.2019.1680788, 91:3, (445-459), Online publication date: 2-Jul-2020. Stankova E, Kruchinina O, Shepovalnikov A and Galperina E (2020) Evolution of the Central Mechanisms of Oral Speech, Journal of Evolutionary Biochemistry and Physiology, 10.1134/S0022093020030011, 56:3, (179-194), Online publication date: 1-May-2020. Gordon A, Geddert R, Hogeveen J, Krug M, Obhi S and Solomon M (2020) Not So Automatic Imitation: Expectation of Incongruence Reduces Interference in Both Autism Spectrum Disorder and Typical Development, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 10.1007/s10803-019-04355-9, 50:4, (1310-1323), Online publication date: 1-Apr-2020. Kool V and Agrawal R (2020) The Evolution of Nonviolence and Its Neurological Basis Gandhi and the Psychology of Nonviolence, Volume 1, 10.1007/978-3-030-56865-8_5, (125-166), . Quinn K (2019) Something Is Going to Happen Here, Advances in Nursing Science, 10.1097/ANS.0000000000000242, 42:3, (E1-E19), Online publication date: 1-Jul-2019. Hayes L (2019) PARIESA: Practice and Research in Enactive Sonic Art Mobile Brain-Body Imaging and the Neuroscience of Art, Innovation and Creativity, 10.1007/978-3-030-24326-5_7, (53-60), . Campbell M, Mehrkanoon S and Cunnington R (2018) Intentionally not imitating: Insula cortex engaged for top-down control of action mirroring, Neuropsychologia, 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.037, 111, (241-251), Online publication date: 1-Mar-2018. Hernandez-Lallement J, van Wingerden M and Kalenscher T (2018) Towards an animal model of callousness, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.029, 91, (121-129), Online publication date: 1-Aug-2018. Ferrari P and Coudé G (2018) Mirror Neurons, Embodied Emotions, and Empathy Neuronal Correlates of Empathy, 10.1016/B978-0-12-805397-3.00006-1, (67-77), . Knapska E and Meyza K (2018) Future Directions, Outstanding Questions Neuronal Correlates of Empathy, 10.1016/B978-0-12-805397-3.00015-2, (191-196), . Whiten A and van de Waal E (2017) Social learning, culture and the 'socio-cultural brain' of human and non-human primates, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.018, 82, (58-75), Online publication date: 1-Nov-2017. Rogers S, Vivanti G and Rocha M (2017) Helping Young Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder Develop Social Ability: The Early Start Denver Model Approach Handbook of Social Skills and Autism Spectrum Disorder, 10.1007/978-3-319-62995-7_13, (197-222), . Meyza K, Bartal I, Monfils M, Panksepp J and Knapska E (2017) The roots of empathy: Through the lens of rodent models, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.028, 76, (216-234), Online publication date: 1-May-2017. Gerson S, Meyer M, Hunnius S and Bekkering H (2017) Unravelling the contributions of motor experience and conceptual knowledge in action perception: A training study, Scientific Reports, 10.1038/srep46761, 7:1, Online publication date: 1-Sep-2017. Eskenazi P, Hartmann F and Rietdijk W (2016) Why controllers compromise on their fiduciary duties: EEG evidence on the role of the human mirror neuron system, Accounting, Organizations and Society, 10.1016/j.aos.2016.02.003, 50, (41-50), Online publication date: 1-Apr-2016. Wurm M, Ariani G, Greenlee M and Lingnau A (2015) Decoding Concrete and Abstract Action Representations During Explicit and Implicit Conceptual Processing, Cerebral Cortex, 10.1093/cercor/bhv169, 26:8, (3390-3401), Online publication date: 1-Aug-2016. Brincker M (2015) Beyond sensorimotor segregation: On mirror neurons and social affordance space tracking, Cognitive Systems Research, 10.1016/j.cogsys.2015.07.002, 34-35, (18-34), Online publication date: 1-Jun-2015. Lamm C and Majdandžić J (2015) The role of shared neural activations, mirror neurons, and morality in empathy – A critical comment, Neuroscience Research, 10.1016/j.neures.2014.10.008, 90, (15-24), Online publication date: 1-Jan-2015. Herrera Morban D and Montero Cruz N (2016) Copying the development: mirror neurons in child development, Medwave, 10.5867/medwave.2016.05.6466, 16:05, (e6466-e6466) Quarta E, Cohen E, Bravi R and Minciacchi D (2020) Future Portrait of the Athletic Brain: Mechanistic Understanding of Human Sport Performance Via Animal Neurophysiology of Motor Behavior, Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 10.3389/fnsys.2020.596200, 14 Ferrari P and Coudé G (2017) Mirror Neurons The International Encyclopedia of Primatology, 10.1002/9781119179313.wbprim0411, (1-7) Maffei R, Convertini L, Quatraro S, Ressa S and Velasco A (2015) Contributions to a neurophysiology of meaning: the interpretation of written messages could be an automatic stimulus-reaction mechanism before becoming conscious processing of information, PeerJ, 10.7717/peerj.1361, 3, (e1361) Graïc J, Peruffo A, Corain L, Centelleghe C, Granato A, Zanellato E and Cozzi B (2020) Asymmetry in the Cytoarchitecture of the Area 44 Homolog of the Brain of the Chimpanzee Pan troglodytes, Frontiers in Neuroanatomy, 10.3389/fnana.2020.00055, 14 This Issue05 June 2014Volume 369Issue 1644Theme Issue 'Mirror neurons: fundamental discoveries, theoretical perspectives and clinical implications' compiled and edited by Pier Francesco Ferrari and Giacomo Rizzolatti Article InformationDOI:https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0169PubMed:24778369Published by:Royal SocietyPrint ISSN:0962-8436Online ISSN:1471-2970History: Published online05/06/2014Published in print05/06/2014 License:© 2014 The Author(s) Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. Citations and impact Keywordsunderstanding otherssocial developmentempathymotor cognitionepigenetics Subjectsbehaviourcognitionneuroscience

Referência(s)