Artigo Revisado por pares

THE ROLE OF IMITATION IN THE STABILIZATION OF HANDEDNESS DURING INFANCY

2006; Imperial College Press; Volume: 05; Issue: 04 Linguagem: Inglês

10.1142/s0219635206001343

ISSN

1757-448X

Autores

Jacqueline Fagard, C. Lemoine,

Tópico(s)

Hemispheric Asymmetry in Neuroscience

Resumo

Journal of Integrative NeuroscienceVol. 05, No. 04, pp. 519-533 (2006) Short CommunicationsNo AccessTHE ROLE OF IMITATION IN THE STABILIZATION OF HANDEDNESS DURING INFANCYJACQUELINE FAGARD and CHRISTELLE LEMOINEJACQUELINE FAGARDLaboratoire Psychologie de la Perception, UFR Biomédicale des Saints Pères, 45 rue des Sts Pères, 75270 Paris Cedex, 06, FranceCorresponding author. and CHRISTELLE LEMOINELaboratoire Cognition et Comportement, 71 avenue, Edouard Vaillant 92774, Boulogne-Billancourt Cedex, Francehttps://doi.org/10.1142/S0219635206001343Cited by:18 PreviousNext AboutSectionsPDF/EPUB ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack CitationsRecommend to Library ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinked InRedditEmail AbstractOnly about 50% of infants appear to be right-handed in their first year, yet only 10–12% ultimately become left-handed. Several parameters may control early changes in handedness, and we investigated the role of imitation. We wanted to see if infants, when choosing the hand to manipulate an object, were influenced by the hand used by the experimenter when demonstrating the target action. We observed infants, seated either opposite or on the lap of the experimenter who used either left or right hand. The results show that when a left-handed experimenter demonstrated the action, none of the infants consistently used a right-handed strategy to manipulate the objects, regardless of the position (lap vs. opposite), and even though most infants had shown right-handedness when first grasping the objects. Imitation may thus be part of the parameters that stabilize infant right-handedness, or, on the contrary, that drive infants towards left-handedness.Keywords:Handednessimitationinfant References E. Abravanel, E. Levan Goldschmidt and M. B. Stevenson, Child Dev. 47, 1032 (1976). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google Scholar M. Annett , Left, right, hand and brain: The right shift theory ( Lawrence Erlbaum Associates , London , 1985 ) . Google ScholarL. Aziz-Zadehet al., Exp. Brain Res. 144, 127 (2002). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarF. Bellagamba and M. Tomasello, Infant Behavior and Development 22, 277 (1999). Crossref, ISI, Google ScholarJ. L. Bradshawet al., Psychol. Res. 56, 170 (1994). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarD. F. Carlson and L. J. Harris, Infant Mental Health Journal 6, 158 (1985). Crossref, Google ScholarL. Carter Saltzman, Science 209, 1263 (1980). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarC. Chiarello, M. A. McMahon and K. Schaefer, Brain Cogn. 11, 18 (1989). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarR. L. Collins, Cerebral Lateralization in Nonhuman Species, ed. S. D. Glick (Academic Press, New York, 1985) pp. 41–71. Crossref, Google ScholarD. Corbetta and E. Thelen, J. Exper. Psych: Human Perception and Performance 22, 502 (1996). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarD. Corbetta and E. Thelen, Dev. Psychobio. 34, 237 (1999). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google Scholar D. Corbetta and E. Thelen , Handbook of Neuropsychology , eds. S. J. Segalowitz and I. Rapin ( Elsevier , 2002 ) . Google ScholarK. S. Cornwell, L. J. Harris and H. E. Fitzgerald, Dev. Neuropsychology 7, 19 (1991). Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar J. F. Coryell and G. F. Michel , Infant Behavior and Dev. 1 , 245 . Crossref, Google ScholarR. Dahmen and J. Fagard, Cortex 6, 739 (1978). Google ScholarM. De Agostini and G. Dellatolas, Developmental Neuropsychology 20, 429 (2001). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarJ. Fagard, The Psychobiology of the Hand, Clinics in Developmental Medicine, ed. K. J. Connolly (MacKeith Press, Londres, 1998) pp. 123–143. Google ScholarJ. Fagard and A. Pezé, J. Motor Behavior 29, 199 (1997). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google Scholar J. Fagard and A. Streri , Droitiers/Gauchers: des asymétries dans tous les sens ("Right-handers/Left-handers: Asymmetries Everywhere") , ed. J. Fagard ( Solal , Marseille , 2004 ) . Google ScholarC. Franckset al., Am. J. Hum. Genet. 72, 499 (2003). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarC. Franckset al., Am. J. Hum. Genet. 70, 800 (2002). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarC. Gabbard and C. R. Helbig, Exp. Brain Res. 156, 325 (2004). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarC. Gabbard, M. Iteya and C. Rabb, Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 51, 176 (1997). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarG. Gergely, H. Bekkering and I. Kiraly, Nature 415, 6873 (2002). Google ScholarE. C. Goldfield, Dynamical Approaches to Development: Applications, eds. L. B. Smith and E. Thelen (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1993) pp. 51–70. Google ScholarJ. Grèzeset al., Neuroimage 18, 928 (2003). Crossref, Medline, Google ScholarD. A. Harkins and G. F. Michel, Developmental Psychobiology 21, 535 (1988). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarD. A. Harkins and I. C. Uzgiris, Infant Behavior and Development 14, 289 (1991). Crossref, ISI, Google ScholarM. Hassler, D. Gupta and H. Wollmann, Int. J. Neurosci. 65, 45 (1992). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarM. Hausmann, Neuropsychologia 43, 1559 (2005). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarP. R. Hawn and L. J. Harris, Manual Specialization and the Developing Brain, eds. G. Younget al. (Academic Press, New York, 1983) pp. 331–348. Crossref, Google ScholarM. Heimann and A. N. Meltzoff, Brit. J. of Dev. Psych. 14, 55 (1996). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarM. Iacoboniet al., Science 286, 2526 (1999). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarL. Koskiet al., J. Neurophysiol. 89, 460 (2003). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarP. Leconte and J. Fagard, Dev. Psychobiol. 45, 51 (2004). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarP. Leconte and J. Fagard, Brain Cogn. 60, 88 (2006). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarD. J. Lewkowicz and G. Turkewitz, Dev. Psych. 18, 301 (1982). Crossref, ISI, Google ScholarG. A. Mathewset al., Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 810 (2004). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarC. M. McCormick and D. M. Maurer, Infant Behavior and Dev. 11, 21 (1988). Crossref, ISI, Google ScholarI. C. McManus and M. P. Bryden, Handbook of Neuropsychology, Vol. 6. Handbook of Neuropsychology, eds. I. Rapin and S. J. Segalowitz (Elsevier Science, New York, 1992) pp. 115–144. Google ScholarI. C. McManuset al., Bri. J. Dev. Psych. 6, 257 (1988). Crossref, ISI, Google ScholarL. A. Mead and E. Hampson, Horm Behav. 31, 65 (1997). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarA. N. Meltzoff and K. Moore, Science 198, 74 (1977). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarA. N. Meltzoff, Child Dev. 59, 217 (1988). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarG. F. Michel, Behavior Genetics 22, 163 (1992). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarG. F. Michel and D. A. Harkins, Dev. Psychobio. 2, 29 (1986). Medline, Google ScholarG. F. Michel, M. R. Ovrut and D. A. Harkins, Genetic, Social and General Psych. Monographs 111, 409 (1985). Google ScholarN. Nishitani and R. Hari, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 97, 913 (2000). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarM. Peters, Manual Specialization and the Developing Brain, eds. G. Younget al. (Academic Press, New York, 1983) pp. 367–374. Crossref, Google Scholar Piaget J, Les théories de l'imitation ("Imitation theories"), 1935 . Google Scholar R. R. Poznanski , Biophysical Neural Networks: Foundations of Integrative Neuroscience , ed. R. R. Poznanski ( Mary Ann Liebert , New York , 2001 ) . Google ScholarF. H. Previc, Psycho. Rev. 98, 299 (1991). Crossref, Google ScholarD. S. Ramsay, Infant Behavior and Dev. 3, 377 (1980). Crossref, ISI, Google ScholarG. Rizzolattiet al., Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 131 (1996). Crossref, Medline, Google ScholarP. Rochat, J. Mot. Behav. 24, 210 (1992). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarC. Rode, M. Wagner and O. Güntürkün, Neuropsychologia 33, 855 (1995). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarP. L. Roubertouxet al., Genetics 163, 1023 (2003). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarA. Shimizu and M. Endo, Cortex 19, 265 (1983). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarT. Sunet al., Science 308, 1794 (2005). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarD. E. Thompson and J. Russell, Dev. Psychol. 40, 882 (2004). Crossref, Medline, ISI, Google ScholarL. Wolpert, European Rev. 13, 97 (2005). Crossref, Google Scholar FiguresReferencesRelatedDetailsCited By 18The Intricate Web of Asymmetric Processing of Social Stimuli in HumansDaniele Marzoli, Anita D'Anselmo, Gianluca Malatesta, Chiara Lucafò and Giulia Prete et al.27 May 2022 | Symmetry, Vol. 14, No. 6Is Handedness at Five Associated with Prenatal Factors?Jacqueline Fagard, Maria De Agostini, Viviane Huet, Lionel Granjon and Barbara Heude29 March 2021 | International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, Vol. 18, No. 7Human Lateralization, Maternal Effects and Neurodevelopmental DisordersGianluca Malatesta, Daniele Marzoli, Giulia Prete and Luca Tommasi22 March 2021 | Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, Vol. 15Measuring infant handedness reliably from reaching: A systematic reviewEliza L. Nelson and Sandy L. Gonzalez16 February 2020 | Laterality, Vol. 25, No. 4Effects of Handedness and Viewpoint on the Imitation of Origami-MakingNatalie Uomini and Rebecca Lawson6 September 2017 | Symmetry, Vol. 9, No. 9Comment le geste vient à l'enfantJacqueline Fagard19 Apr 2017 | Contraste, Vol. N° 45, No. 1Moving What Is Seen: Arm Visibility Modulates Infants' Manual PreferenceLívia Silveira Pogetti, Rosana Machado de Souza, Eloísa Tudella and Luis Augusto Teixeira4 August 2014 | Developmental Neuropsychology, Vol. 39, No. 5Effect of handedness on the occurrence of semantic N400 priming effect in 18- and 24-month-old childrenJacqueline Fagard, Louah Sirri and Pia Rämä28 April 2014 | Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 5Strength of Infants' Bimanual Reaching Patterns is Related to the Onset of Upright LocomotionOsnat Atun-Einy, Sarah E. Berger, Jennifer Ducz and Anat Sher29 July 2013 | Infancy, Vol. 19, No. 1La latéralité manuelle préhistorique, les outils et le langage1Natalie Uomini and John Gowlett30 December 2013 | Revue de primatologie, No. 5Handedness in infants' tool useLauriane Rat-Fischer, J. Kevin O'Regan and Jacqueline Fagard4 September 2012 | Developmental Psychobiology, Vol. 55, No. 8Early development of hand preference and language lateralization: Are they linked, and if so, how?Jacqueline Fagard25 May 2013 | Developmental Psychobiology, Vol. 55, No. 6The nature and nurture of human infant hand preferenceJacqueline Fagard25 April 2013 | Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, Vol. 1288, No. 1Is early manual preference in infants defined by intermanual performance asymmetry in reaching?Rosana Machado Souza, Raymundo Machado de Azevedo Neto, Eloisa Tudella and Luis Augusto Teixeira1 Dec 2012 | Infant Behavior and Development, Vol. 35, No. 4Aux origines de la préférence manuelleJacqueline Fagard5 April 2012 | Enfance, Vol. 2012, No. 01Development of hand preference for object-directed actions and pointing gestures: A longitudinal study between 15 and 25 months of ageHélène Cochet8 June 2011 | Developmental Psychobiology, Vol. 54, No. 1Is infant holding-side bias related to motor asymmetries in mother and child?Céline Scola and Jacques Vauclair7 April 2010 | Developmental Psychobiology, Vol. 52, No. 5Genetic effects on infant handedness under spatial constraint conditionsKunitake Suzuki, Juko Ando and Naho Satou1 Dec 2009 | Developmental Psychobiology, Vol. 51, No. 8 Recommended Vol. 05, No. 04 Metrics History Received 12 October 2006 Accepted 1 December 2006 KeywordsHandednessimitationinfantPDF download

Referência(s)