Revisão Acesso aberto Revisado por pares

Anxiety and Decision-Making

2012; Elsevier BV; Volume: 72; Issue: 2 Linguagem: Inglês

10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.12.027

ISSN

1873-2402

Autores

Catherine A. Hartley, Elizabeth A. Phelps,

Tópico(s)

Decision-Making and Behavioral Economics

Resumo

Although the everyday decision-making of clinically anxious individuals is clearly influenced by their excessive fear and worry, the relationship between anxiety and decision-making remains relatively unexplored in neuroeconomic studies. In this review, we attempt to explore the role of anxiety in decision-making with a neuroeconomic approach. We first review the neural systems mediating fear and anxiety, which overlap with a network of brain regions implicated in studies of economic decision-making. We then discuss the potential influence of cognitive biases associated with anxiety upon economic choice, focusing on a set of decision-making biases involving choice in the face of potential aversive outcomes. We propose that the neural circuitry supporting fear learning and regulation may mediate the influence of anxiety upon choice and suggest that techniques for altering fear and anxiety may also change decisions. Although the everyday decision-making of clinically anxious individuals is clearly influenced by their excessive fear and worry, the relationship between anxiety and decision-making remains relatively unexplored in neuroeconomic studies. In this review, we attempt to explore the role of anxiety in decision-making with a neuroeconomic approach. We first review the neural systems mediating fear and anxiety, which overlap with a network of brain regions implicated in studies of economic decision-making. We then discuss the potential influence of cognitive biases associated with anxiety upon economic choice, focusing on a set of decision-making biases involving choice in the face of potential aversive outcomes. We propose that the neural circuitry supporting fear learning and regulation may mediate the influence of anxiety upon choice and suggest that techniques for altering fear and anxiety may also change decisions. The tendency to experience anxiety is a relatively consistent individual trait (1Spielberger C.D. Anxiety: Current Trends in Theory and Research: I. Academic Press, Oxford, England1972Google Scholar), suggesting that it has stable underlying neural substrates and may be an important factor driving behavioral variation in a variety of domains, including decision-making. In patients suffering from anxiety disorders, heightened anxiety interferes with the ability to adaptively function in everyday tasks, such as employment or social relations. Although it is clear for these patients that their pathological anxiety influences their daily decisions, a more nuanced understanding of the relationship between anxiety and decision-making is needed. Although anxiety has long been known to involve behavioral aberrations in the face of potential negative outcomes, the burgeoning field of neuroeconomics provides a structured approach to studying the computational and neurobiological mechanisms underlying this dysfunction. Neuroeconomic studies typically define mathematically the optimal or normative behavior in a decision-making task, allowing precise quantification of individual deviations from these norms. These parameters can then be used to probe the neural correlates of decision biases. Characterizing the specific decision biases occurring with anxiety may enhance our understanding of the consequences of individual variability in nonclinical trait anxiety as well as the nature of the dysfunction underlying anxiety disorders. In this review, we build on behavioral and neuroeconomic principles of decision-making to explore the impact of anxiety on specific decision variables. We first review the neural systems implicated in fear and anxiety, which overlap with those highlighted in neuroeconomic studies of decision-making. We then discuss how anxiety-induced alterations in the brain circuitry of fear result in predictable cognitive biases that may influence later choices. We review the relatively few studies using behavioral economic paradigms to characterize how anxiety influences decision variables, speculating as to how the effects of anxiety on choice may arise from cognitive biases associated with anxiety and the neural circuitry implicated in fear. Finally, we discuss how techniques for altering fear and anxiety may also change decisions. Fear and anxiety share many common cognitive and physiological properties; however, they can also be distinguished (2Sylvers P. Laprarie J. Lilienfeld S. Differences between trait fear and trait anxiety: Implications for psychopathology.Clin Psychol Rev. 2011; 31: 122-137Crossref PubMed Scopus (242) Google Scholar). Fear responses are elicited by specific stimuli and tend to be short-lived, decreasing once a threat has dissipated. Anxiety may be experienced in the absence of a direct physical threat and typically persists over a longer period of time. However, anxiety is commonly conceptualized as a state of sustained fear (3Davis M. Walker D.L. Miles L. Grillon C. Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety.Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35: 105-135Crossref PubMed Scopus (1069) Google Scholar). Studies of fear neurocircuitry highlight a network of brain regions enabling the adaptive expression of fear to potential threats and its inhibition and control with safety. Details of this circuitry have been investigated across species using classical fear conditioning as a model paradigm. During fear conditioning, a previously neutral stimulus, such as a tone, is paired with an intrinsically aversive stimulus, such as an electric shock, eliciting a range of automatic, unconditioned fear responses (4Davis M. The role of the amygdala in conditioned and unconditioned fear and anxiety.in: Aggleton J.P. The Amygdala. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK2000: 213-288Google Scholar, 5LeDoux J.E. The amygdala.Curr Biol. 2007; 17: 868-874Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (1031) Google Scholar). After one or more tone-shock pairings, presentation of the tone alone is sufficient to elicit a fear response, the conditioned response, providing evidence of a learned association between the two stimuli. Once acquired, conditioned fear can be diminished via a number of techniques (6Hartley C.A. Phelps E.A. Changing fear: The neurocircuitry of emotion regulation.Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35: 136-146Crossref PubMed Scopus (369) Google Scholar). In extinction, the tone is presented repeatedly without the shock, resulting in a gradual decrease in conditioned fear expression. Evidence that fear can return after successful extinction training after the passage of time (spontaneous recovery), changes in context (renewal), or stress (reinstatement) suggests the original fear memory is not erased with extinction but is rather inhibited during the expression of extinction learning (7Bouton M.E. Context and behavioral processes in extinction.Learn Mem. 2004; 11: 485-494Crossref PubMed Scopus (1394) Google Scholar). In humans, intentional cognitive strategies including emotional suppression, redeployment of attention, or cognitive reinterpretation or reappraisal of the significance of a stimulus may also be used to diminish fear (6Hartley C.A. Phelps E.A. Changing fear: The neurocircuitry of emotion regulation.Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35: 136-146Crossref PubMed Scopus (369) Google Scholar). The neurocircuitry supporting fear conditioning has been extensively investigated in animal models and humans (4Davis M. The role of the amygdala in conditioned and unconditioned fear and anxiety.in: Aggleton J.P. The Amygdala. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK2000: 213-288Google Scholar, 5LeDoux J.E. The amygdala.Curr Biol. 2007; 17: 868-874Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (1031) Google Scholar, 8Delgado M.R. Olsson A. Phelps E.A. Extending animal models of fear conditioning to humans.Biol Psychology. 2006; 73: 39-48Crossref PubMed Scopus (287) Google Scholar) and highlights the central role of the amygdala in fear acquisition, storage, and expression. The amygdala is thought to be the site of association and memory storage for simple cued fear conditioning, with projections to the brainstem and hypothalamus mediating autonomic fear expression and projections to the ventral striatum mediating the use of actions to cope with fear (5LeDoux J.E. The amygdala.Curr Biol. 2007; 17: 868-874Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (1031) Google Scholar). In addition, the hippocampus plays an important role in contextual modulation of fear, supporting the acquisition of fears to contexts and guiding the contextually dependent expression of fear (9Fanselow M.S. Contextual fear, gestalt memories, and the hippocampus.Behav Brain Res. 2000; 110: 73-81Crossref PubMed Scopus (604) Google Scholar). Although their specific roles are less clearly defined, the insula and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex are also proposed to modulate fear acquisition (10Shi C. Davis M. Pain pathways involved in fear conditioning measured with fear-potentiated startle: Lesion studies.J Neurosci. 1999; 19: 420-430PubMed Google Scholar, 11Milad M.R. Quirk G.J. Pitman R.K. Orr S.P. Fischl B. Rauch S.L. A role for the human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in fear expression.Biol Psychiatry. 2007; 62: 1191-1194Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (378) Google Scholar). The inhibition or control of conditioned fear requires the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), which is necessary for the storage of extinction memory. During extinction retrieval, projections from the vmPFC to inhibitory interneurons within the amygdala diminish fear expression. After extinction, contextual information modulates the competition between the original fear memory and the new extinction memory (7Bouton M.E. Context and behavioral processes in extinction.Learn Mem. 2004; 11: 485-494Crossref PubMed Scopus (1394) Google Scholar). Projections from the hippocampus to the vmPFC and the amygdala appear to mediate this context-dependent expression of extinction (9Fanselow M.S. Contextual fear, gestalt memories, and the hippocampus.Behav Brain Res. 2000; 110: 73-81Crossref PubMed Scopus (604) Google Scholar, 12Ji J. Maren S. Electrolytic lesions of the dorsal hippocampus disrupt renewal of conditional fear after extinction.Learn Mem. 2005; 12: 270-276Crossref PubMed Scopus (136) Google Scholar). During the intentional, cognitive regulation of fear (and negative affect more generally) amygdala activation typically decreases, driven by increased activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) that, in turn, recruits the vmPFC–amygdala inhibitory pathway that mediates extinction retrieval (6Hartley C.A. Phelps E.A. Changing fear: The neurocircuitry of emotion regulation.Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35: 136-146Crossref PubMed Scopus (369) Google Scholar, 13Ochsner K.N. Gross J.J. The cognitive control of emotion.Trends Cogn Sci. 2005; 9: 242-249Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (3172) Google Scholar, 14Schiller D. Delgado M.R. Overlapping neural systems mediating extinction reversal and regulation of fear.Trends Cogn Sci. 2010; 14: 268-276Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (225) Google Scholar). In short, the amygdala, the vmPFC, and the hippocampus collectively support the acquisition, storage, retrieval, and contextual modulation of fear acquisition and extinction (5LeDoux J.E. The amygdala.Curr Biol. 2007; 17: 868-874Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (1031) Google Scholar, 15Quirk G.J. Mueller D. Neural mechanisms of extinction learning and retrieval.Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008; 33: 56-72Crossref PubMed Scopus (1284) Google Scholar). Although anxiety can be distinguished from fear in a number of important respects (2Sylvers P. Laprarie J. Lilienfeld S. Differences between trait fear and trait anxiety: Implications for psychopathology.Clin Psychol Rev. 2011; 31: 122-137Crossref PubMed Scopus (242) Google Scholar), several prominent theories propose that dysregulation of the neurocircuitry implicated in the acquisition and modulation of conditioned fear may be critically involved in the etiology and maintenance of anxiety (16Rachman S. Neo-conditioning and the classical theory of fear acquisition.Clin Psychol Rev. 1991; 11: 155-173Crossref Scopus (171) Google Scholar, 17Bouton M.E. Mineka S. Barlow D.H. A modern learning theory perspective on the etiology of panic disorder.Psychol Rev. 2001; 108: 4-32Crossref PubMed Scopus (820) Google Scholar, 18Mineka S. Zinbarg R. A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders—It's not what you thought it was.Am Psychol. 2006; 61: 10-26Crossref PubMed Scopus (687) Google Scholar). Neuroimaging studies suggest that the circuitry involved in the learning and regulation of conditioned fear is systematically altered in trait anxious individuals and clinical populations. Trait anxiety is associated with heightened amygdala activation as well as elevated fear expression during fear acquisition (19Lissek S. Powers A.S. McClure E.B. Phelps E.A. Woldehawariat G. Grillon C. et al.Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis.Behav Res Ther. 2005; 43: 1391-1424Crossref PubMed Scopus (779) Google Scholar, 20Indovina I. Robbins T. Nunez-Elizalde A. Dunn B. Bishop S.J. Fear-Conditioning Mechanisms Associated with Trait Vulnerability to Anxiety.Neuron. 2011; 69: 563-571Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (243) Google Scholar). Anxiety also impairs extinction learning and retention (19Lissek S. Powers A.S. McClure E.B. Phelps E.A. Woldehawariat G. Grillon C. et al.Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis.Behav Res Ther. 2005; 43: 1391-1424Crossref PubMed Scopus (779) Google Scholar, 20Indovina I. Robbins T. Nunez-Elizalde A. Dunn B. Bishop S.J. Fear-Conditioning Mechanisms Associated with Trait Vulnerability to Anxiety.Neuron. 2011; 69: 563-571Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (243) Google Scholar, 21Rauch S.L. Milad M.R. Orr S.P. Quinn B.T. Fischl B. Pitman R.K. Orbitofrontal thickness, retention of fear extinction, and extraversion.Neuroreport. 2005; 16: 1909-1912Crossref PubMed Scopus (118) Google Scholar) as well as regulation of emotional responses via intentional cognitive strategies (22Amstadter A. Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders.J Anxiety Disord. 2008; 22: 211-221Crossref PubMed Scopus (228) Google Scholar, 23Cisler J.M. Koster E.H.W. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review.Clin Psychol Rev. 2010; 30: 203-216Crossref PubMed Scopus (1165) Google Scholar). These deficits appear to stem from impairments in the prefrontal-amygdala circuitry that typically supports the regulation of fear expression. Anxious individuals exhibit reduced prefrontal activation during or before fear extinction (20Indovina I. Robbins T. Nunez-Elizalde A. Dunn B. Bishop S.J. Fear-Conditioning Mechanisms Associated with Trait Vulnerability to Anxiety.Neuron. 2011; 69: 563-571Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (243) Google Scholar, 24Sehlmeyer C. Dannlowski U. Schöning S. Kugel H. Pyka M. Pfleiderer B. et al.Neural correlates of trait anxiety in fear extinction.Psychol Med. 2011; 41: 789-7981Crossref PubMed Scopus (125) Google Scholar) and require heightened prefrontal recruitment to successfully reduce negative emotion through cognitive reappraisal (25Campbell-Sills L. Barlow D.H. Brown T.A. Hofmann S.G. Effects of suppression and acceptance of emotional responses of individuals with anxiety and mood disorders.Behav Res Ther. 2005; 44: 1251-1263Crossref PubMed Scopus (565) Google Scholar). Anatomical evidence suggests prefrontal inhibition of the amygdala is mediated primarily by a fiber tract from the vmPFC to inhibitory cells within the amygdala (26Ghashghaei H.T. Hilgetag C.C. Barbas H. Sequence of information processing for emotions based on the anatomic dialogue between prefrontal cortex and amygdala.Neuroimage. 2007; 34: 905-923Crossref PubMed Scopus (649) Google Scholar). Structural integrity of this vmPFC–amygdala pathway is inversely correlated with trait anxiety (27Kim M.J. Whalen P.J. The structural integrity of an amygdala-prefrontal pathway predicts trait anxiety.J Neurosci. 2009; 29: 11614-11618Crossref PubMed Scopus (347) Google Scholar), suggesting that anatomically compromised inhibitory function contributes to heightened reactivity and impaired emotion regulation in anxiety. Finally, atrophy of the hippocampus in clinically anxious patients (28Bremner J.D. Randall P. Scott T.M. Bronen R.A. Seibyl J.P. Southwick S.M. et al.MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder.Am J Psychiatry. 1995; 152: 973-981PubMed Google Scholar) suggests that contextual modulation of fear may also be altered in anxiety. Consistent with this hypothesis, clinically anxious individuals show increased generalization of conditioned fear to similar stimuli (29Lissek S. Rabin S. Heller R.E. Lukenbaugh D. Geraci M. Pine D.S. Grillon C. Overgeneralization of conditioned fear as a pathogenic marker of panic disorder.Am J Psychiatry. 2010; 167: 47-55Crossref PubMed Scopus (420) Google Scholar). Additional brain regions may contribute to differences in emotional expression and awareness associated with anxiety. Although the amygdala clearly mediates cue-evoked phasic fear responses to threat-related stimuli, the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, a region in the ventral basal forebrain referred to as part of the “extended amygdala,” appears to support a more sustained state of arousal and vigilance characteristic of anxious individuals (3Davis M. Walker D.L. Miles L. Grillon C. Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety.Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35: 105-135Crossref PubMed Scopus (1069) Google Scholar, 30Somerville L.H. Whalen P.J. Kelley W.M. Human bed nucleus of the stria terminalis indexes hypervigilant threat monitoring.Biol Psychiatry. 2010; 68: 416-424Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (230) Google Scholar). Anxiety is associated with heightened perception of physiological bodily sensations, or interoception (31Paulus M.P. Stein M.B. Interoception in anxiety and depression.Brain Struct Funct. 2010; 214: 451-463Crossref PubMed Scopus (561) Google Scholar), which may increase the aversiveness of responses to threats (32Reiss S. Peterson R.A. Gursky D.M. McNally R.J. Anxiety sensitivity, anxiety frequency and the prediction of fearfulness.Behav Res Ther. 1986; 24: 1-8Crossref PubMed Scopus (2638) Google Scholar). The insula appears to play a critical role in the representation of interoceptive information (33Craig A.D. How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and human awareness.Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009; 10: 59-70Crossref PubMed Scopus (4582) Google Scholar). Increased interoceptive awareness in anxious individuals appears to be mediated by altered insula reactivity and is thought to contribute to the maintenance of anxiety (31Paulus M.P. Stein M.B. Interoception in anxiety and depression.Brain Struct Funct. 2010; 214: 451-463Crossref PubMed Scopus (561) Google Scholar). The neurocircuitry of fear and anxiety provides a basis for understanding how anxiety may alter decision-making. Neuroeconomic studies of decision-making have highlighted a network of brain regions including the striatum, amygdala, vmPFC, insula, and dlPFC (34Rangel A. Camerer C. Montague R. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision-making.Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008; 9: 545-556Crossref PubMed Scopus (1421) Google Scholar) that are also implicated in the expression and control of fear (4Davis M. The role of the amygdala in conditioned and unconditioned fear and anxiety.in: Aggleton J.P. The Amygdala. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK2000: 213-288Google Scholar, 5LeDoux J.E. The amygdala.Curr Biol. 2007; 17: 868-874Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (1031) Google Scholar, 6Hartley C.A. Phelps E.A. Changing fear: The neurocircuitry of emotion regulation.Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010; 35: 136-146Crossref PubMed Scopus (369) Google Scholar, 7Bouton M.E. Context and behavioral processes in extinction.Learn Mem. 2004; 11: 485-494Crossref PubMed Scopus (1394) Google Scholar). Although precisely how these shared networks jointly contribute to anxiety and decision-making is unclear, this overlap suggests the brain systems mediating fear and anxiety are intertwined with those underlying the computation of value and choice. Although it is not surprising that dysregulation of the fear conditioning neurocircuitry has robust effects on emotional processing in anxious individuals, recent neuroimaging research suggests alterations in cognitive processing typically observed in anxiety may share the same underlying neural substrates (35Bishop S.J. Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention.Nat Neurosci. 2009; 12: 92-98Crossref PubMed Scopus (607) Google Scholar). A large body of research highlights two principal information-processing biases characteristic of anxiety: 1) a bias to attend toward threat-related information, and 2) a bias toward negative interpretation of ambiguous stimuli (36Mathews A. MacLeod C. Cognitive vulnerability to emotional disorders.Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2005; 1: 167-195Crossref PubMed Scopus (1399) Google Scholar). Across a variety of tasks, anxiety is associated with a general pattern of faster response times when detecting a threat stimulus or identifying a target cued by a threat stimulus and slower response times when detecting a neutral stimulus or reporting neutral information in the presence of a threat stimulus (23Cisler J.M. Koster E.H.W. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review.Clin Psychol Rev. 2010; 30: 203-216Crossref PubMed Scopus (1165) Google Scholar, 37Mogg K. Bradley B.P. A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety.Behav Res Ther. 1998; 36: 809-848Crossref PubMed Scopus (1297) Google Scholar, 38Bar-Haim Y. Lamy D. Pergamin L. Bakermans-Kranenburg van IJzendoorn M.H. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non-anxious individuals: A meta-analytic study.Psychol Bull. 2007; 133: 1-24Crossref PubMed Scopus (2735) Google Scholar). This attentional bias appears to reflect both facilitated detection of threat-related stimuli and difficulty in disengaging attention from negative stimuli, relative to neutral or positive stimuli (23Cisler J.M. Koster E.H.W. Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in anxiety disorders: An integrative review.Clin Psychol Rev. 2010; 30: 203-216Crossref PubMed Scopus (1165) Google Scholar). For stimuli with more than one potential interpretation, anxiety is associated with a tendency toward a more negative perception. For instance, anxious individuals tend to interpret ambiguous emotional facial expressions (39Richards A. French C.C. Calder A.J. Webb B. Fox R. Young A.W. Anxiety-related bias in the classification of emotionally ambiguous facial expressions.Emotion. 2002; 2: 273-287Crossref PubMed Scopus (157) Google Scholar), face–voice pairings (40Koizumi A. Tanaka A. Imai H. Hiramatsu S. Hiramoto E. Sato T. et al.The effects of anxiety on the interpretation of emotion in the face-voice pairs.Exp Brain Res. 2011; 213: 275-282Crossref PubMed Scopus (23) Google Scholar), and homophones (e.g., “die/dye”) (41Eysenck M.W. MacLeod C. Matthews A. Cognitive functioning and anxiety.Psychol Res. 1987; 49: 189-195Crossref PubMed Scopus (213) Google Scholar) as more negative in valence than less-anxious individuals. When evaluating the outcome probabilities of ambiguous future life events, anxious individuals unrealistically judge negative outcomes to be more likely than positive ones (42Butler G. Mathews A. Anticipatory anxiety and risk perception.Cogn Ther Res. 1987; 11: 551-565Crossref Scopus (262) Google Scholar, 43MacLeod A. Williams J.M.G. Bekerian D.A. Worry is reasonable: The role of explanations in pessimism about future personal events.J Abnorm Psychol. 1991; 100: 478-486Crossref PubMed Scopus (167) Google Scholar, 44Eysenck M.W. Derakshan N. Cognitive biases for future negative events as a function of trait anxiety and social desirability.Person Indiv Diff. 1997; 22: 597-605Crossref Scopus (59) Google Scholar). Studies indicating that patients with anxiety disorders report negative biases in the interpretation of disorder-related stimuli (45Richards J.C. Austin D.W. Alvarenga M.E. Interpretation of ambiguous interoceptive stimuli in panic disorder and nonclinical panic.Cogn Ther Res. 2001; 25: 235-246Crossref Scopus (61) Google Scholar, 46Amir N. Beard C. Bower E. Interpretation bias and social anxiety.Cogn Ther Res. 2005; 29: 433-443Crossref Scopus (115) Google Scholar, 47Yoon K.L. Zinbarg R.E. Interpreting neutral faces as threatening is a default mode for socially anxious individuals.J Abnorm Psychol. 2008; 117: 680-685Crossref PubMed Scopus (138) Google Scholar) suggest that this bias may be selectively applied to self-relevant information. Biased attention to threat in anxious individuals is proposed to reflect both engagement of pre-attentive amygdala-dependent threat evaluation processes (48Mathews A. Mackintosh B. A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety.Cogn Ther Res. 1998; 22: 539-560Crossref Scopus (562) Google Scholar) and compromised prefrontal control mechanisms typically engaged during attentional competition and control (49Bishop S.J. Jenkins R. Lawrence A.D. Neural processing of fearful faces: Effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations.Cereb Cortex. 2007; 17: 1595-1603Crossref PubMed Scopus (281) Google Scholar). Consistent with this proposal, high trait anxiety is associated with increased amygdala activity to attended as well as unattended threat stimuli (50Bishop S.J. Duncan J. Lawrence A.D. State anxiety modulation of the amygdala response to unattended threat-related stimuli.J Neurosci. 2004; 24: 10364-10368Crossref PubMed Scopus (402) Google Scholar, 51Etkin A. Klemenhagen K.C. Dudman J.T. Rogan M.T. Hen R. Kandel E.R. Hirsch J. Individual differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful faces.Neuron. 2004; 44: 1043-1055Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (494) Google Scholar, 52Dickie E.W. Armony J.L. Amygdala responses to unattended fearful faces: Interaction between sex and trait anxiety.Psychiatry Res. 2008; 162: 51-57Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (72) Google Scholar) and decreased prefrontal activation under conditions of attention competition (49Bishop S.J. Jenkins R. Lawrence A.D. Neural processing of fearful faces: Effects of anxiety are gated by perceptual capacity limitations.Cereb Cortex. 2007; 17: 1595-1603Crossref PubMed Scopus (281) Google Scholar, 50Bishop S.J. Duncan J. Lawrence A.D. State anxiety modulation of the amygdala response to unattended threat-related stimuli.J Neurosci. 2004; 24: 10364-10368Crossref PubMed Scopus (402) Google Scholar), even in the absence of threat-related stimuli (35Bishop S.J. Trait anxiety and impoverished prefrontal control of attention.Nat Neurosci. 2009; 12: 92-98Crossref PubMed Scopus (607) Google Scholar). The amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC) also appear to contribute to the negative interpretation bias in anxiety. In healthy individuals, the magnitude of the amygdala blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) signal to ambiguous surprise facial expressions is positively correlated with the degree to which the expression is interpreted as negative as opposed to positive (53Kim H. Somerville L.H. Johnstone T. Alexander A.L. Whalen P.J. Inverse amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to surprised faces.Neuroreport. 2003; 14: 2317-2322Crossref PubMed Scopus (281) Google Scholar). Higher trait anxiety is associated with heightened amygdala BOLD responses during passive viewing of neutral faces (54Somerville L.H. Kim H. Johnstone T. Alexander A.L. Whalen P.J. Human amygdala responses during presentation of happy and neutral faces: Correlations with state anxiety.Biol Psychiatry. 2004; 55: 897-903Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (210) Google Scholar) and a tendency to interpret neutral faces more negatively (47Yoon K.L. Zinbarg R.E. Interpreting neutral faces as threatening is a default mode for socially anxious individuals.J Abnorm Psychol. 2008; 117: 680-685Crossref PubMed Scopus (138) Google Scholar). A study in mice reporting greater amygdala responsivity and anxiety-like behavior in the context of temporally unpredictable neutral stimuli suggests the amygdala may play a more general role in mediating an anxiogenic response to ambiguity (55Herry C. Bach D.R. Esposito F. Di Salle F. Perrig W.J. Scheffler K. et al.Processing of temporal unpredictability in human and animal amygdala.J Neurosci. 2007; 27: 5958-5966Crossref PubMed Scopus (317) Google Scholar). In contrast, regions of the PFC appear to support intentional efforts to reinterpret negative stimuli more positively (13Ochsner K.N. Gross J.J. The cognitive control of emotion.Trends Cogn Sci. 2005; 9: 242-249Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (3172) Google Scholar, 56Wager T.D. Davidson M.L. Hughes B.L. Lindquist M.A. Ochsner K.N. Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation.Neuron. 2008; 59: 1037-1050Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (1190) Google Scholar) as well as the automatic effects of positive contextual information on interpretation of ambiguous facial expressions (57Kim H. Somerville L.H. Johnstone T. Polis S. Alexander A.L. Shin L.M. et al.Contextual modulation of amygdala responsivity to surprised faces.J Cog Neurosci. 2004; 16: 1730-1745Crossref PubMed Scopus (322) Google Scholar). Collectively, these data suggest that amygdala hyperresponsivity while attending to, evalua

Referência(s)