Artigo Acesso aberto Revisado por pares

Rorschach’s Idea of a “Movement” Response in the Light of Recent Philosophy and Psychology of Perception

2000; Hogrefe Verlag; Volume: 24; Issue: 1 Linguagem: Inglês

10.1027/1192-5604.24.1.1

ISSN

2151-206X

Autores

Helge Malmgren,

Tópico(s)

Hemispheric Asymmetry in Neuroscience

Resumo

Original ArticleRorschach's Idea of a "Movement" Response in the Light of Recent Philosophy and Psychology of PerceptionHelge MalmgrenHelge Malmgren Department of Philosophy, Göteborg University, Sweden Published Online:September 07, 2011https://doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604.24.1.1PDFView Full Text ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinkedInReddit SectionsMoreReferences Aglioti, S. , DeSouza, J. F. , Goodale, M. A. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–685. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Alin, L. (1994). The effect of motor and cognitive inhibition on the movement responses in the Rorschach test (94:1). Göteborg: Applied Psychology. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Arnheim, R. (1974). Art and visual perception. (2 ed.). Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Bardy, B. G. , Warren, W. H. (1997). Visual control of braking in goal-directed action and sport. Journal of Sports Sciences, 15. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Bastian, C. (1880). The brain as an organ of the mind. London: Parker. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Berkeley, G. (1901). An Essay toward a New Theory of Vision. In A. C. Fraser, (Ed.), The works of George Berkeley (Vol. I, pp. 93–210). Oxford: Clarendon. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Bermúdez, J. L. (1998). The paradox of self-consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Bleuler, E. (1921). Naturgeschichte der Seele und ihres Bewusstwerdens. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Bohm, E. (1972). Lehrbuch der Rorschachpsychodiagnostik. (4 ed.). Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Bruce, V. , Green, P. , Georgeson, M. (1966). Visual perception. (3 ed.). Hove: Psychology Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Bullock, D. , Cisek, P. , Grossberg, S. (1998). Cortical networks for control of voluntary arm movements under variable force conditions. Cerebral Cortex, 8, 48–62. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Carey, D. P. , Dijkerman, H. C. , Milner, A. D. (1998). Perception and action in depth. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 438–453. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Chatterjee, S. H. , Freyd, J. , Shiffrar, M. (1996). Configural processing in the perception of apparent biological motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 22, 916–929. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Ellenberger, H. (1954). The life and work of Hermann Rorschach (1884–1922). Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 18, 172–219. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Ellenberger, H. (1993). The life and work of Hermann Rorschach (1884–1922). In M. Micale, (Ed.), Beyond the Unconscious. Essays of Henri F. Ellenberger in the history of psychiatry (pp. 192–236). Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Exner, J. E. (1991). Comments on "The Rorschach M Response: A Return to its Roots." Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 37–41. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Exner, J. E. (1993). The Rorschach: A comprehensive system. (3 ed.). (Vol. 1). New York: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Gallagher, S. (1995). Body schema and intentionality. In J. L. Bermúdez, A. J. Marcel, N. Eilan, (Eds.), The body and the self (pp. 225–244). Cambridge: MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Gibson, E. J. , Walk, R. D. (1960). The visual cliff. Scientific American, 202, 64–70. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Goldstein, F. , Levin, H. (1995). Post-traumatic and anterograde amnesia following closed head injury. In A. Baddeley, B. Wilson, F. Watts, (Eds.), Memory disorders (pp. 187–210). Chichester: Wiley. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Goodale, M. A. , Milner, A. D. , Jakobson, L. S. , Carey, D. P. (1991). Perceiving the world and grasping it. A neurological dissociation. Nature, 349, 154–156. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Haffenden, A. M. , Goodale, M. A. (1998). The effect of pictorial illusion on prehension and perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 122–136. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Helms Tillery, S. I. , Soechting, J. F. , Ebner, T. J. (1996). Somatosensory activity in relation to arm posture: Nonuniform spatial tuning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 78, 2423–2437. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Jeannerod, M. (1997). The cognitive neuroscience of action. Oxford: Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Kramer, R. L. (1991). The Rorschach M response: A return to its roots. Journal of Personality Assessment, 57, 30–36. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Lasslop, P. (1976). Kinästhesie. In J. Ritter, K. Gründer, (Eds.), Historisches Wörterbuch der Philosophie (Vol. 4, pp. 819–827). Basel & Stuttgart: Schwabe. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality: Selected papers. New York: MaGraw-Hill. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Lindqvist, G. , Malmgren, H. (1990). Organisk psykiatri. Stockholm: Almqvist & Wiksell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Malmgren, H. , Östensson, O. (1989). Selective attention is selective learning. Göteborg Psychological Reports, 19(2). First citation in articleGoogle Scholar McCloskey, D. I. (1981). Corollary discharge: motor commands and perception. In V. B. Brooks, (Ed.), Handbook of physiology, Section 1: The nervous system (Vol. 2, pp. 1415–1448). Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Meltzoff, A. N. , Moore, M. K. (1995). Infants' understanding of people and things: From body imitation to folk psychology. In J. L. Bermúdez, (Ed.), The body and the self (pp. 43–70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945). Phénoménologie de la perception. Paris. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Micale, M. (Ed.). (1993). Beyond the unconscious. Essays by Henri F. Ellenberger. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton U.P. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Millikan, R. (1996). Pushmi-pullyu representations. In J. Tomberlin, (Ed.), Philosophical perspectives (Vol. IX, pp. 185–200). Atascadero, CA: Ridgeview Publishing. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Mourly Vold, J. (1900). Ueber "Hallucinationen," vorzüglich "Gesichtshallucinationen," auf der Grundlage von cutan-motorischen Zuständen und auf derjenigen von vergangenen Gesichts-Eindrücken. Allgemeine Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie, 57, 834–865. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Mourly Vold, J. (1910–12). Über den Traum. Leipzig: Barth. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Nelson, R. J. (1996). Interactions between motor commands and somatic perception in sensorimotor cortex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, 801–810. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Paulignan, Y. , MacKenzie, C. , Martiniuk, R. , Jeannerod, M. (1991). Selective pertuberation of visual input during prehension movement. I. Experimental Brain Research, 83, 502–512. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Pellegrino, G. D. , Fadiga, L. , Fogassi, L. , Gallese, V. , Rizzolati, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Redström, J. (1998). Affordances, information pickup on direct perception. Göteborg: Philosophy. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Rorschach, H. (1912). Über "Reflexhallucinationen" und verwandte Erscheinungen. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 30, 357–400. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Rorschach, H. (1972). Psychodiagnostik (9th ed.). Bern: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Scheerer, E. (1987). Muscle sense and innervation feelings: A chapter in the history of perception and action. In H. Heuer, A. Sanders, (Eds.), Perspectives on perception and action (pp. 171–194). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Sperry, R. (1959). Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetc response produced by visual neural inversion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 482–489. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar von Holst, E. , Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip (Wechselwirkungen zwischen Zentralnervensystem und Peripherie). Die Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464– 476. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Wertheimer,. (1912). Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 61, 161–265. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Williams, J. G. (1988). Perception of a throwing action from point-light demonstrations. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 67, 273–274. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Wittgenstein, L. (1967). Zettel (G.E.M. Anscombe, Trans.). Oxford: Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Wittgenstein, L. (1968). Philosophische Untersuchungen/Philosophical investigations (G.E.M. Anscombe, Trans.). Oxford: Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarFiguresReferencesRelatedDetailsCited byInkblots as a cultural phenomenon: On the centenary of the Rorschach test21 January 2022 | Current Problems of Psychiatry, Vol. 22, No. 4Therapeutic exploration with the Rorschach Inkblot Test: a case demonstration of the lifeworld approach with a child2 August 2021 | Asia Pacific Journal of Counselling and Psychotherapy, Vol. 12, No. 2Movement responses in the Rorschach test. Part 1 Testing healthy and schizophrenic individuals12 July 2019 | Current Problems of Psychiatry, Vol. 20, No. 1The Effects of Neurological Priming on the Rorschach A Pilot Experiment on the Human Movement ResponseLuciano Giromini, Donald J. Viglione, Emanuela Brusadelli, Alessandro Zennaro, Marzia Di Girolamo, and Piero Porcelli10 June 2016 | Rorschachiana, Vol. 37, No. 1The "Feeling of Movement": Notes on the Rorschach Human Movement Response11 November 2015 | Journal of Personality Assessment, Vol. 98, No. 2Mirroring Activity in the Brain and Movement Determinant in the Rorschach TestJournal of Personality Assessment, Vol. 95, No. 5A Norwegian in Freud's works: John Mourly Void21 March 2013 | The Scandinavian Psychoanalytic Review, Vol. 35, No. 2 Volume 24Issue 1January 2000ISSN: 1192-5604eISSN: 2151-206X InformationRorschachiana (2000), 24, pp. 1-27 https://doi.org/10.1027/1192-5604.24.1.1.© 2000Hogrefe & Huber PublishersAcknowledgments:This study was supported by a grant from the Swedish Council for Research in the Humanities and Social Sciences (grant no. F0588/96). I want to thank Sven Carlsson, Thorild Dahlquist, Elisabeth Engman, Gösta Fröbärj, Kristina Malmgren, Susanna Radovic, Johan Redström, Fredrik Sundqvist and Christer Svennerlind for valuable criticism.PDF download

Referência(s)