Examining the Distinct Effects of Emotive Triggers on Public Reactions to International Terrorism
2013; Taylor & Francis; Volume: 25; Issue: 5 Linguagem: Inglês
10.1080/09546553.2012.692739
ISSN1556-1836
AutoresÇiğdem V. Şirin, Nehemia Geva,
Tópico(s)Misinformation and Its Impacts
ResumoAbstract In recent years, a growing body of research has set out to examine the role that emotions play in shaping political attitudes and behaviors regarding terrorism. However, one major issue that is generally overlooked is whether the thematic relevance of emotive triggers leads to differential effects on people's reactions to international terrorism. Specifically, does anger—regardless of its source—tend to drive people towards supporting an aggressive foreign policy option to counter terrorism, or do the thematic underpinnings of anger (i.e., the specific contents that trigger this particular emotion, such as watching a news story about a recent terrorist attack) matter vis-à-vis the policy choice? To address this gap, this study experimentally examines the impact of anger—induced by thematically relevant versus irrelevant emotive triggers—on people's cognitive processing and foreign policy preferences regarding international terrorism. Overall, we find that the induction of anger via thematically relevant emotive triggers leads to a higher tendency for selecting a military option, a lower amount of information acquisition, and a shorter processing time in response to terror-related incidents. Keywords: emotionsinformation processingpolicy preferencespublic opinionterrorism Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank José D. Villalobos and the three anonymous reviewers for their instructive comments and suggestions. We would also like to thank Uri Geva for programming the DecTracer. Notes Note: *p ≤ .10; **p ≤ .05. The dependent variable is coded as 0 = Evacuation, 1 = Military targeting. Robust standard errors are in parentheses. The emotion-neutral control condition is the reference category. Anne Aly and Lelia Green, “Fear, Anxiety and the State of Terror,” Studies in Conflict & Terrorism 33, no. 3 (2010): 268–281; Daniel Bar-Tal and Daniela Labin, “The Effect of a Major Event on Stereotyping: Terrorist Attacks in Israel and Israeli Adolescents’ Perceptions of Palestinians, Jordanians and Arabs,” European Journal of Social Psychology 31, no. 3 (2001): 265–280; Darren Davis and Brian Silver, “Civil Liberties vs. Security: Public Opinion in the Context of the Terrorist Attacks on America,” American Journal of Political Science 48, no. 1 (2004): 28–47; Nehemia Friedland and Ariel Merari, “The Psychological Impact of Terrorism: A Double-Edged Sword,” Political Psychology 6, no. 4 (1985): 591–604; Shana K. Gadarian, “The Politics of Threat: How Terrorism News Shapes Foreign Policy Attitudes,” Journal of Politics 72, no. 2 (2010): 469–483; Leonie Huddy, Stanley Feldman, Gallya Lahav, and Charles Taber, “Fear and Terrorism: Psychological Reactions to 9/11,” in Pippa Norris, Montague Kern, and Marion Just, eds., Framing Terrorism: The News Media, the Government, and the Public (New York: Routledge, 2003), 255–280; Jennifer Merolla and Elizabeth Zechmeister, Democracy at Risk: How Terrorist Threats Affect the Public (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2009); and Melody S. Sadler, Megan Lineberger, Joshua Correll, and Bernadette Park, “Emotions, Attributions, and Policy Endorsements in Response to the September 11th Terrorist Attacks,” Basic and Applied Social Psychology 27, no. 3 (2005): 249–258. Leonie Huddy, Stanley Feldman, Charles Taber, and Gallya Lahav, “Threat, Anxiety, and Support of Anti-Terrorism Policies,” American Journal of Political Science 49, no. 3 (2005): 610–625; Jennifer S. Lerner, Roxana M. Gonzalez, Deborah A. Small, and Baruch Fischhoff, “Effects of Fear and Anger on Perceived Risks of Terrorism: A National Field Experiment,” Psychological Science 14, no. 2 (2003): 144–150; and Linda J. Skitka, Christopher W. Bauman, Nicholas P. Aramovich, and G. Scott Morgan, “Confrontational and Preventative Policy Responses to Terrorism: Anger Wants a Fight and Fear Wants ‘Them’ to Go Away,” Basic and Applied Social Psychology 28, no. 4 (2006): 375–384. Ibid. One should note that there is no universally accepted definition of international terrorism. Nevertheless, the literature contains several useful conceptualizations that offer some common ground to build on for operational purposes. For instance, Jenkins posits that international terrorism consists of terror acts with clear international repercussions such as incidents in which terrorists operate abroad to strike their targets, select certain targets because of their connections to a foreign state, or attack international lines of commerce. On a parallel basis, Wilkinson defines international terrorism as “political terrorism directed at foreign targets; concerted by factions of more than one state; or aimed at influencing the policies of a foreign government.” For the purposes of this study, we focus on acts of international terrorism in a country targeting the citizens and interests of another country. Therein, because the subject pool of our experiment was drawn from the United States, the specific context of the study concerns the reactions of the U.S. public to terrorist attacks abroad targeting U.S. citizens and interests. See Brian M. Jenkins, The Study of Terrorism: Definitional Problems (Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation, 1980), 3; Paul Wilkinson, Terrorism and the Liberal State (London: Macmillan, 1986), 182. See also Walter Enders and Todd Sandler, The Political Economy of Terrorism (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press, 2006). Michael A. Cohn, Matthias R. Mehl, and James W. Pennebaker, “Linguistic Markers of Psychological Change Surrounding September 11, 2001,” Psychological Science 15, no. 10 (2004): 687–693; Barbara L. Fredrickson, Michele M. Tugade, Christian E. Waugh, and Gregory R. Larkin, “What Good are Positive Emotions in Crisis? A Prospective Study of Resilience and Emotions following the Terrorist Attacks on the United States on September 11, 2001,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84, no. 2 (2003): 365–376; Mark J. Landau, Sheldon Solomon, Jeff Greenberg, Florette Cohen, Tom Pyszczynski, Jamie Arndt, et al., “Deliver Us from Evil: The Effects of Mortality Salience and Reminders of 9/11 on Support for President George W. Bush,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30, no. 9 (2004): 1136–1150; Matthias R. Mehl and James W. Pennebaker, “The Social Dynamics of a Cultural Upheaval: Social Interactions Surrounding September 11, 2001,” Psychological Science 14, no. 6 (2003): 579–585; Roxane Cohen Silver, E. Alison Holman, Daniel N. McIntosh, Michael Poulin, and Virginia Gil-Rivas, “Nationwide Longitudinal Study of Psychological Responses to September 11,” Journal of the American Medical Association 288, no. 10 (2002): 1235–1244; Michael Traugott, Ted Brader, Deborah Coral, Richard Curtin, David Featherman, Robert Groves, et al., “How Americans Responded: A Study of Public Reactions to 9/11/01,” PS: Political Science and Politics 35, no. 3 (2002): 511–516. Deborah A. Small, Jennifer S. Lerner, and Baruch Fischhoff, “Emotion Priming and Attributions for Terrorism: Americans’ Reactions in a National Field Experiment,” Political Psychology 27, no. 2 (2006): 289–298. Baruch Fischhoff, Roxana M. Gonzalez, Deborah A. Small, and Jennifer S. Lerner, “Evolving Judgments of Terror Risks: Foresight, Hindsight, and Emotion,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 11, no. 2 (2005): 124–139; Lerner et al. (see note 2 above); Mosi Rosenboim, Uri Benzion, Shosh Shahrabani, and Tal Shavit, “Emotions, Risk Perceptions, and Precautionary Behavior under the Threat of Terror Attacks: A Field Study Among Israeli College Students,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 25, no. 3 (2012): 248–256. Friedland and Merari (see note 1 above); Huddy et al. (see note 2 above); Skitka et al. (see note 2 above). Davis and Silver (see note 1 above); Leonie Huddy, Stanley Feldman, and Christopher Weber, “The Political Consequences of Perceived Threat and Felt Insecurity,” Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 614, no. 1 (2007): 131–153. Leonie Huddy, Stanley Feldman, Theresa Capelos, and Colin Provost, “The Consequences of Terrorism: Disentangling the Effects of Personal and National Threat,” Political Psychology 23, no. 3 (2002): 485–509. Krista De Castella and Craig McGarty, “Two Leaders, Two Wars: A Psychological Analysis of Fear and Anger Content in Political Rhetoric about Terrorism,” Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy 11, no. 1 (2011): 180–200; Krista De Castella, Craig McGarty, and Luke Musgrove, “Fear Arousing Content in Political Rhetoric About Terrorism: An Analysis of Speeches by Australian Prime Minister Howard,” Political Psychology 30, no. 1 (2009): 1–26; and John Mueller, Overblown: How Politicians and the Terrorism Industry Inflate National Security Threats, and Why We Believe Them (New York: Free Press, 2006). Gadarian (see note 1 above); and Anat Shoshani and Michelle Slone, “The Drama of Media Coverage of Terrorism: Emotional and Attitudinal Impact on the Audience,” Studies in Conflict & Terrorism 31, no. 7 (2008): 627–640. Huddy et al. (see note 1 above); Landau et al. (see note 5 above). Kimberly Gross, Paul R. Brewer, and Sean Aday, “Confidence in Government and Emotional Responses to Terrorism after September 11, 2001,” American Politics Research 37, no. 1 (2009): 107–128. Brad M. Hastings and Barbara A. Shaffer, “Authoritarianism and Sociopolitical Attitudes in Response to Threats of Terror,” Psychological Reports 97, no. 2 (2005): 623–630. Linda J. Skitka, Christopher W. Bauman, and Elizabeth Mullen, “Political Tolerance and Coming to Psychological Closure following September 11, 2001: An Integrative Approach,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 30, no. 6 (2004): 743–756. Huddy et al. (see note 2 above). See, for example, Eric J. Johnson and Amos Tversky, “Affect, Generalization, and the Perception of Risk,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45, no. 1 (1983): 20–31; and David Watson, David Wiese, Jatin Vaidya, and Auke Tellegen, “The Two General Activation Systems of Affect: Structural Findings, Evolutionary Considerations, and Psychobiological Evidence,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 76, no. 5 (1999): 820–838. See Leonie Huddy, Stanley Feldman, and Erin Cassese, “On the Distinct Political Effects of Anxiety and Anger,” in W. Russell Neuman, George E. Marcus, Ann Crigler, and Michael MacKuen, eds., The Affect Effect: Dynamics of Emotion in Political Thinking and Behavior (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2007), 204. Norbert Schwarz and Gerald L. Clore, “Mood, Misattribution, and Judgments of Well-Being: Informative and Directive Functions of Affective States,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 45, no. 3 (1983): 513–523; Norbert Schwarz and Gerald L. Clore, “Feelings and Phenomenal Experiences,” in E. Tory Higgins and Arie W. Kruglanski, eds., Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles (New York: Guilford Press, 1996), 433–465. The affect heuristic is also similar to the model of “risk as feelings” that Loewenstein et al. have developed to explain people's instinctive reactions to risky and/or dangerous situations. Paul Slovic, Melissa L. Finucane, Ellen Peters, and Donald G. MacGregor, “The Affect Heuristic,” European Journal of Operational Research 177, no. 3 (2007): 1333–1352; and George F. Loewenstein, Elke U. Weber, Christopher K. Hsee, and Ned Welch, “Risk as Feelings,” Psychological Bulletin 127, no. 2 (2001): 267–286. See, for example, Galen V. Bodenhausen, Lori A. Sheppard, and Geoffrey P. Kramer, “Negative Affect and Social Judgment: The Differential Impact of Anger and Sadness,” European Journal of Social Psychology 24, no. 1 (1994): 45–62; David DeSteno, Richard E. Petty, Duane T. Wegener, and Derek D. Rucker, “Beyond Valence in the Perception of Likelihood: The Role of Emotion Specificity,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78, no. 4 (2000): 397–416; Huddy et al. (see note 19 above); Jennifer S. Lerner and Dacher Keltner, “Beyond Valence: Toward a Model of Emotion Specific Influences on Judgment and Choice,” Cognition and Emotion 14, no. 4 (2000): 473–493; and George E. Marcus, W. Russell Neuman, and Michael Mackuen, Affective Intelligence and Political Judgment (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2000). Lerner and Keltner (see note 22 above). See also Richard S. Lazarus, Emotion and Adaptation (New York: Oxford University Press, 1991); Jennifer S. Lerner and Dacher Keltner, “Fear, Anger, and Risk,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81, no. 1 (2001): 146–159; and Craig A. Smith and Phoebe C. Ellsworth, “Patterns of Cognitive Appraisal in Emotion,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48, no. 4 (1985): 813–838. Bodenhausen et al. (see note 22 above); James Druckman and Rose McDermott, “Emotion and the Framing of Risky Choice,” Political Behavior 30, no. 3 (2008): 297–321; Lerner and Keltner (see note 22 above); Jennifer Lerner and Larissa Z. Tiedens, “Portrait of the Angry Decision Maker: How Appraisal Tendencies Shape Anger's Influence on Cognition,” Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 19, no. 2 (2006): 115–137; Marcus et al. (see note 22 above); Larissa Z. Tiedens, “The Effect of Anger on the Hostile Inferences of Aggressive and Non-Aggressive People: Specific Emotions, Cognitive Processing, and Chronic Accessibility,” Motivation and Emotion 25, no. 3 (2001): 233–251; and Larissa Z. Tiedens and Susan Linton, “Judgment under Emotional Certainty and Uncertainty: The Effects of Specific Emotions on Information Processing,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81, no. 6 (2001): 973–988. Huddy et al. (see note 22 above); Lerner et al. (see note 2 above); Skitka et al. (see note 2 above). See, for example, James R. Averill, “Studies on Anger and Aggression: Implications for Theories of Emotion,” American Psychologist 38, no. 11 (1983): 1145–1160; Robin L. Nabi, “A Cognitive Functional Model for the Effects of Discrete Negative Emotions on Information Processing, Attitude Change and Recall,” Communication Theory 9, no. 3 (1999): 292–320; and Carol Tavris, Anger: The Misunderstood Emotion (New York: Simon & Schuster, 1982), cited in Monique Mitchell Turner, “Using Emotion in Risk Communication: The Anger Activism Model,” Public Relations Review 33, no. 2 (2007): 114–119. Ibid. Small et al. (see note 6 above). Lerner et al. (see note 2 above). See also Fischhoff et al. (see note 7 above). Skitka et al. (see note 2 above). Karen Gasper and Gerald L. Clore, “The Persistent Use of Negative Affect by Anxious Individuals to Estimate Risk,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74, no. 5 (1998): 1350–1363; Julie H. Goldberg, Jennifer S. Lerner, and Philip E. Tetlock, “Rage and Reason: The Psychology of the Intuitive Prosecutor,” European Journal of Social Psychology 29, nos. 5-6 (1999): 781–795; Schwarz and Clore (see note 20 above). Gasper and Clore (see note 31 above); Goldberg et al. (see note 22 above). See also Johnson and Tversky (see note 18 above). Schwarz and Clore (see note 20 above). See also Dacher Keltner, Kenneth D. Locke, and Paul C. Aurain, “The Influence of Attributions on the Relevance of Negative Feelings to Personal Satisfaction,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 19, no. 1 (1993): 21–29; Norbert Schwarz, Wolfgang Servay, and Martin Kumpf, “Attribution of Arousal as a Mediator of the Effectiveness of Fear-arousing Communications,” Journal of Applied Social Psychology 15, no. 2 (1985): 178–188; and Matthias Siemer and Rainer Reisenzein, “Effects of Mood on Evaluative Judgments: Influence of Reduced Processing Capacity and Mood Salience,” Cognition & Emotion 12, no. 6 (1998): 783–805. Rosenboim et al. (see note 7 above). See also Michel T. Pham, “Emotion and Rationality: A Critical Review and Interpretation of Empirical Evidence,” Review of General Psychology 11, no. 2 (2007): 155–178. Edward R. Hirt, Gary M. Levine, Hugh E. McDonald, and R. Jeffrey Melton, “The Role of Mood in Quantitative and Qualitative Aspects of Performance: Single or Multiple Mechanisms?,” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 33, no. 6 (1997): 602–629; Dacher Keltner, Phoebe C. Ellsworth, and Kari Edwards, “Beyond Simple Pessimism: Effects of Sadness and Anger on Social Perception,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64, no. 5 (1993): 740–752; Michel T. Pham, “Representativeness, Relevance, and the Use of Feelings in Decision Making,” Journal of Consumer Research 25, no. 2 (1998): 144–159; and Robert C. Sinclair, Melvin M. Mark, and Gerald L. Clore, “Mood-Related Persuasion Depends on Misattributions,” Social Cognition 12, no. 4 (1994): 309–326. Our experimental design section offers an extended comparative discussion of public reactions to terrorist attacks versus traffic accidents (particularly in terms of the varying dread/unknown risk characteristics and in further reference to the social amplification of risk framework). See Roger E. Kasperson, Ortwin Renn, Paul Slovic, Halina S. Brown, Jacque Emel, Robert Goble, et al., “The Social Amplification of Risk: A Conceptual Framework,” Risk Analysis 8, no. 2 (1988): 177–187; Paul Slovic, “Perception of Risk,” Science 236, no. 4799 (1987): 280–285; and William J. Burns and Paul Slovic, “The Diffusion of Fear: Modeling Community Response to a Terrorist Strike,” The Journal of Defense Modeling and Simulation: Applications, Methodology, Technology 4, no. 4 (2007): 298–317. Lerner et al. (see note 2 above); Skitka et al. (see note 2 above). Bodenhausen et al. (see note 22 above); Druckman and McDermott (see note 24 above); Lerner and Keltner (see note 22 above); Lerner and Tiedens (see note 24 above); Marcus et al. (see note 22 above); Tiedens (see note 24 above); and Tiedens and Linton (see note 24 above). All experimental materials are available upon request. Doris Graber, Mass Media and American Politics, 2nd ed. (Washington, DC: CQ Press, 1984), 37, cited in John R. Hibbing and Elizabeth Theiss-Morse, “The Media's Role in Public Negativity toward Congress: Distinguishing Emotional Reactions and Cognitive Evaluations,” American Journal of Political Science 42, no. 2 (1998): 475–498. See also Doris Graber, “Say It with Pictures,” Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Sciences 546 (1996): 85–96. Russell Neuman, Marion R. Just, and Ann N. Crigler, Common Knowledge: News and the Construction of Political Meaning (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1992). Deborah A. Small and Jennifer S. Lerner, “Emotional Policy: Personal Sadness and Anger Shape Judgments about a Welfare Case,” Political Psychology 29, no. 2 (2008): 149–168. See also Lerner et al. (see note 2 above); Fritz Strack, Norbert Schwarz, and Elisabeth Gschneidinger, “Happiness and Reminiscing: The Role of Time Perspective, Affect, and Mode of Thinking,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 49, no. 6 (1985): 1460–1469; and Tiedens and Linton (see note 24 above). Donald T. Campbell and Donald W. Fiske, “Convergent and Discriminant Validation by the Multitrait-Multimethod Matrix,” Psychological Bulletin 56, no. 2 (1959): 81–105. Writing exercise questions are adopted from Small and Lerner (see note 42 above). Some studies also add “the number of people exposed to the risk” as a third factor. See Slovic (see note 36 above). Ibid. Burns and Slovic (see note 36 above); M. Brooke Rogers, Richard Amlôt, G. James Rubin, Simon Wessely, and Kristian Krieger, “Mediating the Social and Psychological Impacts of Terrorist Attacks: The Role of Risk Perception and Risk Communication,” International Review of Psychiatry 19, no. 3 (2007): 279–288. Slovic (see note 36 above). Kasperson et al. (see note 36 above), 186. Ibid. Cass R. Sunstein, “Terrorism and Probability Neglect,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 26, nos. 2/3 (2003): 121–136. See also Yuval Rottenstreich and Christopher K. Hsee, “Money, Kisses, and Electric Shocks: On the Affective Psychology of Risk,” Psychological Science 12, no. 3 (2001): 185–190; and Paul Slovic and Ellen Peters, “Risk Perception and Affect,” Current Directions in Psychological Science 15, no. 6 (2006): 322–325. Bruce Bongar, Lisa M. Brown, Larry E. Beutler, James N. Breckenridge, and Philip G. Zimbardo, eds., Psychology of Terrorism (New York: Oxford University Press, 2007). See, for example, Vincent Yzerbyt, Muriel Dumont, Daniel Wigboldus, and Ernestine Gordijn, “I Feel for Us: The Impact of Categorization and Identification on Emotions and Action Tendencies,” British Journal of Social Psychology 42, no. 4 (2003): 533–550. W. Kip Viscusi and Richard J. Zeckhauser, “Sacrificing Civil Liberties to Reduce Terrorism Risks,” Journal of Risk and Uncertainty 26, nos. 2/3 (2003): 99–120. Ibid. See, for example, Brigitte L. Nacos, “Terrorism as Breaking News: Attack on America,” Political Science Quarterly 118, no. 1 (2003): 23–52; Brigitte L. Nacos, Yaeli Bloch-Elkon, and Robert Shapiro, Selling Fear: Counterterrorism, the Media, and Public Opinion (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 2011); and Shoshani and Slone (see note 12 above). Lerner et al. (see note 2 above); Skitka et al. (see note 2 above); Bodenhausen et al. (see note 22 above); Lerner and Tiedens (see note 24 above); Tiedens (see note 24 above); Tiedens and Linton (see note 24 above); and Lerner and Keltner (see note 22 above). As Izard et al. suggest, individuals can experience several emotions at any given moment and one emotion may trigger another. Taking this possibility into consideration, in addition to anger, we asked the participants whether they experienced several other emotions (specifically, fear and sadness) in reaction to the experimental stimuli as part of our manipulation checks. The participants rated these emotions on a scale of 0 to 10. The results demonstrate that the means for anger expressed by participants after exposure to emotive triggers in both the thematically relevant and irrelevant conditions (M R = 7.00 and M IR = 5.65) are substantively higher than the means for fear (M R = 3. 55 and M IR = 2.35) and sadness (M R = 4.95 and M IR = 3.75). The results thus suggest that anger is the dominant emotion induced by the experimental stimuli. It is also important to note that even if other emotions are intervening with the predicted effects of anger, our experimental results would be more, not less, conservative because it would demonstrate that the hypothesized effects for anger still hold even in the presence of other emotional states that would otherwise nullify or mediate the impact of anger. Carroll E. Izard, Brian P. Ackerman, Kristen M. Schoff, and Sarah E. Fine, “Self-Organization of Discrete Emotions, Emotion Patterns, and Emotion-Cognition Relations,” in Marc D. Lewis and Isabela Granic, eds., Emotion, Development, and Self-Organization (New York: Cambridge University Press, 2000), 15–36. See also Sadler et al. (see note 1 above). See, for example, Laurence G. Grimm, Statistical Applications for the Behavioral Sciences (New York: Wiley, 1993). See, for example, Burns and Slovic (see note 36 above); Kasperson et al. (see note 36 above); Rogers et al. (see note 47 above); and Sunstein (see note 51 above). Reuben M. Baron and David A. Kenny, “The Moderator-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psychological Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Considerations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51, no. 6 (1986): 1173–1182. David P. MacKinnon and James H. Dwyer, “Estimating Mediated Effects in Prevention Studies,” Evaluation Review 17, no. 2 (1993): 144–158. See De Castella and McGarty (see note 11 above); and De Castella et al. 2009 (see note 11 above). See also Susanne Halverscheid and Erich H. Witte, “Justification of War and Terrorism: A Comparative Case Study Analyzing Ethical Positions Based on Prescriptive Attribution Theory,” Social Psychology 39, no. 1 (2008): 26–36; and Mueller (see note 11 above). See James N. Breckenridge, Philip G. Zimbardo, and Jennifer L. Sweeton, “After Years of Media Coverage, Can One More Video Report Trigger Heuristic Judgments? A National Study of American Terrorism Risk Perceptions,” Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression 2, no. 3 (2010): 163–178; Jaeho Cho, Michael P. Boyle, Heejo Keum, Mark D. Shevy, Douglas M. McLeod, Dhavan V. Shah, and Zhongdang Pan, “Media, Terrorism, and Emotionality: Emotional Differences in Media Content and Public Reactions to the September 11th Terrorist Attacks,” Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media 47, no. 3 (2003): 309–327; Gadarian (see note 1 above); Nacos (see note 56 above); Nacos et al. (see note 56 above); Pippa Norris, Montague Kern, and Marion Just, eds., Framing Terrorism: The News Media, the Government, and the Public (New York: Routledge, 2003); Shoshani and Slone (see note 12 above); and Michelle Slone, “Responses to Media Coverage of Terrorism,” Journal of Conflict Resolution 44, no. 4 (2000): 508–522. Slone (see note 64 above); and Shoshani and Slone (see note 12 above). Nacos (see note 56 above); and Shoshani and Slone (see note 12 above). See, for example, Shoshani and Slone (see note 12 above). Sheldon Solomon, Jeff Greenberg, and Tom Pyszczynski, “The Cultural Animal: Twenty Years of Terror Management Theory and Research,” in Jeff Greenberg, Sander L. Koole, and Tom Pyszczynski, eds., Handbook of Experimental Existential Psychology (New York: Guilford Press, 2004), 13–24; Daniela Niesta, Immo Fritsche, and Eva Jonas, “Mortality Salience and its Effects on Peace Processes: A Review,” Social Psychology 39, no. 1 (2008): 48–58. Lerner et al. (see note 2 above); Skitka et al. (see note 2 above); Bodenhausen et al. (see note 22 above); Lerner and Tiedens (see note 24 above); Tiedens (see note 24 above); Tiedens and Linton (see note 24 above); and Lerner and Keltner (see note 22 above). Ben Sheppard, “Mitigating Terror and Avoidance Behavior through the Risk Perception Matrix to Augment Resilience,” Journal of Homeland Security and Emergency Management 8, no. 1 (2011): 1. See, for example, Baruch Fischhoff, “Assessing and Communicating the Risks of Terrorism,” in Albert H. Teich, Stephen D. Nelson, and Stephen J. Lita, eds., Science and Technology in a Vulnerable World (Washington, DC: AAAS, 2002), 51–64; and Rogers et al. (see note 47 above). Turner (see note 26 above). See also Monique Mitchell Turner, Rajiv N. Rimal, Daniel Morrison, and Hyojin Kim, “The Role of Anxiety in Seeking and Retaining Risk Information: Testing the Risk Perception Attitude Framework in Two Studies,” Human Communication Research 32, no. 2 (2006): 130–156. Ibid. Ibid., 118. Luke Musgrove and Craig McGarty, “Opinion-Based Group Membership as a Predictor of Collective Emotional Responses and Support for Pro- and Anti-War Action,” Social Psychology 39, no. 1 (2008): 37–47. See, for example, Ole R. Holsti, Public Opinion and American Foreign Policy (Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press, 1996); James A. Nathan and James K. Oliver, “Public Opinion and U.S. Security Policy,” Armed Forces & Society 2, no. 1 (1975): 46–62; Benjamin I. Page and Robert Y. Shapiro, “Effects of Public Opinion on Policy,” American Political Science Review 77, no. 1 (1983): 175–190; and Richard Sobel, The Impact of Public Opinion on U.S. Foreign Policy since Vietnam: Constraining the Colossus (New York: Oxford University Press, 2001). For instance, Sears argues that young adults have “less-crystallized attitudes, less-formulated senses of self, stronger cognitive skills, stronger tendencies to comply with authority, and more unstable peer group relationships.” Such differences may create concern about the generalizability of experimental findings based on student samples. However, by using Monte Carlo simulations on different levels of attitude crystallization, Druckman and Kam statistically demonstrate that “any convenience sample poses a problem only when the size of an experimental treatment effect depends upon a characteristic on which the convenience sample has virtually no variance.” David O. Sears, “College Sophomores in the Laboratory: Influences of a Narrow Data Base on Social Psychology's View of Human Nature,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51, no. 3 (1986): 515; and James N. Druckman and Cindy D. Kam, “Students as Experimental Participants: A Defense of the ‘Narrow Data Base,'” in James N. Druckman, Donald P. Green, James H. Kuklinski, and Arthur Lupia, eds., Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science (Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press, 2011), 41. See also Donald R. Kinder and Thomas R. Palfrey, “On Behalf of Experimental Political Science,” in Donald R. Kinder and Thomas R. Palfrey, eds., Experimental Foundations of Political Science (Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press 1993), 1–42; and Rose McDermott, “Experimental Methods in Political Science,” Annual Review of Political Science 5 (2002): 31–61. Douglas G. Mook, “In Defense of External Invalidity,” American Psychologist 38, no. 4 (1983): 386. See also Rebecca B. Morton and Kenneth C. Williams, Experimental Political Science and the Study of Causality: From Nature to the Lab (Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2010); and Druckman and Kam (see note 77 above). Scott S. Gartner, “The Multiple Effects of Casualties on Public Support for War: An Experimental Approach,” American Political Science Review 102, no. 1 (2008): 95–106. http://tvnews.vanderbilt.edu. Alex Mintz, Steven B. Redd, and Arnold Vedlitz, “Can We Generalize from Student Experiments to the Real World in Political Science, Military Affairs, and International Relations?” Journal of Conflict Resolution 50, no. 5 (2006): 757–776. Druckman and Kam (see note 77 above). See also, for example, Cindy D. Kam, “Who Toes the Party Line? Cues, Values, and Individual Differences,” Political Behavior 27, no. 2 (2005): 163–182; Anton Kühberger, “The Influence of Framing on Risky Decisions: A Meta-analysis,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 75, no. 1 (1998): 23–55; and Joanne M. Miller and Jon A. Krosnick, “News Media Impact on the Ingredients of Presidential Evaluations: Politically Knowledgeable Citizens Are Guided by a Trusted Source,” American Journal of Political Science 44, no. 2 (2000): 301–315. Gartner (see note 79 above). Lars Willnat et al., “Media Use, Anti-Americanism and International Support for the Iraq War,” International Communication Gazette 68, nos. 5–6 (2006): 533–550. Susy Kovatz, Ilan Kutz, Gil Rubin, Rachel Dekel, and Louis Shenkman, “Comparing the Distress of American and Israeli Medical Students Studying in Israel during a Period of Terror,” Medical Education 40, no. 4 (2006): 389–393. Ibid. Friedland and Merari (see note 1 above). See also, for example, Bar-Tal and Labin (see note 1 above); and Carol Gordon and Asher Arian, “Threat and Decision Making,” Journal of Conflict Resolution 45, no. 2 (2001): 196–215. See, for example, Huddy et al. (see note 2 above); Lerner et al. (see note 2 above); Sadler et al. (see note 1 above); and Skitka et al. (see note 2 above). Additional informationNotes on contributorsCigdem V. Sirin Cigdem V. Sirin is an assistant professor in the Department of Political Science at the University of Texas at El Paso. Her main areas of research are international relations and political psychology, with a particular interest in conflict behavior. Nehemia Geva Nehemia Geva is an associate professor in the Department of Political Science at Texas A&M University. He specializes in foreign policy decision making and political cognition.
Referência(s)