Neural Correlates of Automatic and Controlled Auditory Processing in Schizophrenia
2008; American Psychiatric Association Publishing; Volume: 20; Issue: 4 Linguagem: Inglês
10.1176/jnp.2008.20.4.419
ISSN1545-7222
AutoresRajendra A. Morey, Teresa V. Mitchell, Seniha Inan, Jeffrey A. Lieberman, Ayşenil Belger,
Tópico(s)Neural dynamics and brain function
ResumoBack to table of contents Previous article Next article REGULARFull AccessNeural Correlates of Automatic and Controlled Auditory Processing in SchizophreniaRajendra A. Morey M.D., M.S.Teresa V. Mitchell Ph.D.Seniha Inan M.D.Jeffrey A. Lieberman M.D.Aysenil Belger Ph.D.Rajendra A. Morey M.D., M.S.Search for more papers by this authorTeresa V. Mitchell Ph.D.Search for more papers by this authorSeniha Inan M.D.Search for more papers by this authorJeffrey A. Lieberman M.D.Search for more papers by this authorAysenil Belger Ph.D.Search for more papers by this authorPublished Online:1 Oct 2008AboutSectionsPDF/EPUB ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinked InEmail E vidence indicates that both early sensory discrimination of auditory information and higher-order cognitive processing are impaired in individuals with schizophrenia. 1 – 3 Functional imaging studies have revealed reduced activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, 4 – 6 subcortical regions, and limbic areas during controlled (attended) auditory processing, 4 , 6 – 8 while showing reduced superior temporal lobe activity during automatic (unattended) auditory processing. 9 , 10 More generally, hypoactivation in frontal, temporal, and striato-thalamic brain regions is characteristic of schizophrenia. 11 Given the deficits in automatic and controlled auditory information processing in schizophrenia, it is important to compare the neural systems associated with these two aspects of auditory information processing and their changes with illness duration. Electrophysiological indices show differences between automatic and controlled processing that are further modulated by illness duration, 12 but have not been assessed with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The primary goal of this study was to investigate differences in brain function associated with schizophrenia during controlled and automatic auditory processing. A secondary goal was to examine relations between functional brain differences and illness duration. We hypothesized that patients would show impaired prefrontal, superior temporal, and striato-thalamic responses during auditory information processing relative to participants in the comparison group. We further hypothesized that differences in regional brain activation between automatic and controlled auditory processing would be more pronounced in the comparison group relative to the patient group. This prediction was based on evidence that consistently shows that patients with schizophrenia produce lower overall task-related neural activity (i.e., compared to baseline) as well as reduced condition effects across many cognitive domains. 3 , 6 , 13 Furthermore, we predicted that frontal, superior temporal, and striato-thalamic function would deteriorate with illness duration. We employed an auditory oddball task because it elicits activation of frontotemporal regions in healthy comparison subjects, 14 it requires top-down organization and integration of auditory information, 14 , 15 and patients with schizophrenia exhibit impaired task-associated neural function. 4 , 12 , 16METHODParticipants The study participants included 26 patients with schizophrenia and 17 neuro-psychiatrically healthy comparison subjects. Participant groups were matched for age (t=0.61, df=41, p=0.27), gender (χ 2 =0.95, df=1, p=0.33), handedness (χ 2 =2.2, df=1, p=0.14), 17 and race (African-American or European-American) (χ 2 =1.4, df=1, p=0.23). Years of education was calculated based on missing data from four comparison subjects and three schizophrenia patients and was greater in the comparison group (t=3.4, df=35, p<0.001). Demographic and clinical characteristics are summarized in Table 1 . TABLE 1. Subject Sample Demographics and Clinical CharacteristicsTABLE 1. Subject Sample Demographics and Clinical CharacteristicsEnlarge table Diagnoses were confirmed by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID) and rated using the Symptom Onset in Schizophrenia inventory. 18 Patients experiencing acute psychotic symptoms, active substance use disorders, or pregnancy were excluded. Patients were recruited from inpatient and outpatient facilities affiliated with the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. Comparison subjects with a history of psychiatric illness or a first-degree relative with psychotic symptoms were excluded. All participants received a verbal explanation of the study and provided written informed consent for procedures approved by the institutional review boards at UNC-Chapel Hill and Duke University Medical Center. Stimuli and Tasks Participants performed two tasks consecutively in a single session. The first was an automatic auditory detection task in which participants were presented with task-irrelevant frequent standard tones and infrequent pitch-deviant tones (unattended auditory deviants) while performing a visual discrimination task that required a forced choice response between infrequent targets and frequent nontargets. The second was a controlled auditory processing task during which participants responded with a unique button press to infrequently occurring pitch-deviant tones (attended auditory target) and an alternate button press to frequently occurring standard tones. At the same time, participants passively viewed nontarget stimuli (squares) to facilitate objective comparison with the first task. Visual and auditory information were presented synchronously for both tasks. Although different from the traditional oddball paradigm which requires a response only to rare events, the forced-choice paradigm enables examination of reaction time data for all trials and has previously been shown to elicit a homologous pattern of activation to rare events as the target detection version. 19 Subjects completed the unattended auditory deviant (UAD) runs prior to the attended auditory target (AAT) runs in order to minimize possible priming of the UAD tones due to their role in the AAT condition. Exposure to AAT trials prior to the UAD trials could lead to stronger orienting to the UAD stimuli and therefore contaminate a task that was designed to measure pure automatic processing. This kind of orienting and contamination is likely to be greater in comparison subjects than in patients, as suggested by the latent inhibition literature. 20For both tasks, frequently occurring (90%) standard tones (1000 Hz) and infrequently occurring (10%) deviant tones (1064 Hz) tones of 68 ms duration were delivered through headphones (Resonance Technology Inc., Northridge, Calif.) at ∼85 dB with a stimulus onset asynchrony of 1500 ms. Button assignment for target and nontarget stimuli was counterbalanced across participants. Right-handed behavioral responses on a fiber-optic response box were time-locked to fMRI data acquisition for 42 UAD trials over seven runs (∼5 minutes/run; 200 image volumes), followed by 42 AAT trials over three runs (∼4 minutes/run; 160 image volumes). Relevant trials were separated by a random interval of 15 to 18 seconds to enable full recovery of the hemodynamic response to baseline and facilitate event-related fMRI analysis. The UAD trials were spaced further apart to accommodate for trials from the visual discrimination task resulting in a greater number of image volumes corresponding to the UAD trials (200×7 runs) than AAT trials (160×3 runs).Acquisition of MRI DataImages were acquired on a 1.5 Tesla GE Signa scanner with a birdcage type standard quadrature head coil and an advanced nuclear magnetic resonance echo planar system. The participant’s head was positioned along the canthomeatal line and immobilized using a vacuum cushion and a forehead strap. High-resolution T1 weighted anatomical images (3D SPGR, TR =22 ms, TE=5 ms, flip angle =20°, FOV =24 cm, voxel =0.9375×0.9375×1.5 mm, 256×256 voxels, 24 images), two-dimensional spin echo sequence (TR=4000ms, TE=20, flip angle=90°, FOV=24 cm, 1.5 mm slice thickness), and 16 contiguous axial coplanar anatomical images parallel to the anterior commissure-posterior commissure plane coincident with 16 functional slices using a gradient echo planar sequence (TR=1500ms, TE=40ms, flip angle=90°, NEX=1, voxel=3.75×3.75×5 mm, imaging matrix 64×64 voxels) were acquired.Functional Image Analysis Our preliminary analysis method involved the computation of random-effects activation and contrast maps, which was followed up by a confirmatory region of interest analysis method. This approach has been used in many previous studies. 3 , 21 Head motion was detected by center of mass measurements in three orthogonal planes. On the basis of these measurements, runs containing movement greater than 1 mm from baseline were rejected, resulting in the elimination of single runs in one comparison subject and four patients (<1% of the total data). Image preprocessing (slice timing correction, motion correction, coregistration, normalization, smoothing) was performed using SPM99. Post-processing and quantitative analysis were performed using custom Matlab software (Duke-UNC BIAC, Durham, N.C.) using the procedures described. Random Effects Analysis The preliminary analysis consisted of a random-effects assessment of the differences among the conditions and groups based on the event-related hemodynamic response. In a whole-brain voxel-based approach, individual participant t-maps were generated for each target event (AAT and UAD trials) by identifying voxels in which the average activity correlated with an empirically derived hemodynamic response template. First, epochs of 15 consecutive image volumes were extracted from the raw signal coincident with each target event (an AAT or UAD). Each epoch consisted of five image volumes preceding the target event (at −7.5, −6, −4.5, −3, and −1.5 seconds), nine image volumes following the target event (at 1.5, 3, 4.5, 6, 7.5, 9, 10.5, 12, and 13.5 seconds) and the image volume coincident with the target event (at 0 seconds). Epochs were averaged separately for the AAT and UAD conditions. Second, the BOLD signal time series for each voxel was correlated with an empirical reference waveform, and t-statistics were calculated for the correlation coefficients for each voxel. A significance threshold of p 1.96 (p<0.05, uncorrected) were accepted as activated to compute the number of active voxels in the region of interest, then divided by the total number of voxels in the region of interest to obtain the percentage of activated voxels. The average percentage signal change at each time point in the event epoch was calculated within each region of interest. Analysis of the UAD condition was performed on all trials. Analysis of the AAT condition was performed first for all trials, then again for correct trials only. FIGURE 1. An Axial Slice and a 3-D Rendering (Posterior View; Cross Hair at AC) Show the Six Regions of Interest Selected Based on a priori Hypotheses of Automatic and Controlled Auditory ProcessingRegions are color coded: dark blue=anterior cingulate gyrus (ATG), green=inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), red=middle frontal gyrus (middle frontal gyrus), yellow=superior temporal gyrus (STG), orange=basal ganglia (BG), and light blue=thalamus (TH).Correlation Analysis Between Activation and Clinical-Behavioral MeasuresWe investigated the relationship between brain activation (percentage of activated voxels and percentage signal change) for the six regions of interest and clinical illness duration, percent hits on the AAT task, and antipsychotic dosing (chlorpromazine equivalents).RESULTSBehavioral PerformanceIn the attended auditory target (AAT) controlled processing task, patients made fewer hits, detection or correct responses (mean±SE of means; 57.5±5.0%), than the comparison subjects (76.6±5.6%; t=2.5, df=41, p<0.01), but more misses, or false negatives (36.6±5.2%), than comparison subjects (23.5±4.5%; t=1.9, df=41, p<0.05). There were no significant group differences for nonresponses (t=1.1, df=41, p=0.13; patients=5.8±2.3%; comparison=2.3±1.5%), for latency on correct trials (t=1.7, df=41, p=0.43; patients=516±23 msec; comparison=510±28 msec), or for incorrect trials (t=1.1, df=41, p=0.13; patients=414±60 msec; comparison =320±21 msec). During the unattended auditory deviants (UAD) automatic processing task, subjects performed a visual discrimination task. Patients again made fewer hits (54±4%) than comparison subjects (74±4%; t=3.1, df=41, p R), insula, basal ganglia, and thalamus. There were no areas in which activation in the patient group was greater than the comparison group. FIGURE 2. Random-Effects Maps for Contrasts (t>3.6) Between Conditions in the Comparison Group (Upper Panel) and the Schizophrenia Patient Group (Lower Panel)Areas where the contrast for attended auditory target (AAT) > unattended auditory deviant (UAD) are coded in blue, and areas where UAD>AAT are coded in red. Controlled processing showed greater activation in both groups in the bilateral insula (INS). The comparison group showed additional areas with greater activation for controlled processing including the left middle frontal gyrus (MFG), thalamus (TH), dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus (ACG), and anterior and posterior aspects of the left superior temporal gyrus (STG).FIGURE 3. Random-Effects Contrast Maps (t>3.6; Masked by Active Voxels in the Comparison Group) Showing Comparison > Patient ActivationUpper panels=unattended auditory deviant (UAD) condition in superior temporal gyrus (STG), Heschl’s gyrus (primary auditory cortex), and thalamus. Lower panels=random-effects contrast maps showing comparison > patient activation for the attended auditory target (AAT) condition in middle frontal gyrus (MFG), inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), anterior cingulate gyrus (ACG), thalamus (TH), and insula. Controlled auditory processing showed generally greater activation relative to automatic processing (AAT>UAD) in both patients and comparison subjects with a few exceptions. Controlled processing elicited greater activation than automatic processing in both groups in the bilateral insula. Controlled processing also elicited greater activation for comparison subjects in the left middle frontal gyrus, thalamus, dorsal anterior cingulate gyrus, and anterior and posterior aspects of the left superior temporal gyrus. By contrast, automatic processing elicited greater activation than controlled processing in a focal area of the rostral anterior cingulate gyrus for both participant groups, and for the comparison group only, in the anterior and posterior portions of the left superior temporal gyrus (see Figure 2 ). Regions of Interest Analyses The percentage of activated voxels within each anatomical region of interest was analyzed as the dependent variable in an omnibus analysis of variance (ANOVA) with factors of group (patient, comparison), region (ACG, IFG, MFG, STG, basal ganglia, thalamus), condition (AAT, UAD), and hemisphere (Left, Right). This analysis resulted in main effects for group (F=7.8, df=1, 41, p<0.01), region (F=6.5, df=5, 205, p<0.0001], condition (F=40.3, df=1, 41, p<0.0001), and hemisphere (F=21.1, df=1, 41, p<0.0001), as well as interaction effects for region×group (F=2.3, df=5, 205, p<0.05) and region×condition (F=5.1, df=5, 205, p<0.005), and a trend level interaction for region×condition×group (F=2.0, df=5, 205, p<0.08), but no effect for hemisphere×group (F=0.04, df=1, 41, p=0.85). As seen in Figure 4 , post hoc analyses showed greater overall extent of activation in comparison subjects than patients (main effect of group) and greater activation to the AAT condition than UAD condition (main effect of condition). Post hoc analyses of the UAD condition revealed that the patient group had a smaller extent of activation in the superior temporal gyrus (t=4.21, df=41, p<0.05), thalamus (t=5.6, df=41, p<0.05), and middle frontal gyrus (t=4.6, df=41, p<0.05). Post hoc analysis of the AAT condition showed smaller extent of activation in the patient group in inferior frontal gyrus (t=1.7, df=41, p<0.05), basal ganglia (t=2.1, df=41, p<0.02), and thalamus (t=2.5, df=41, p<0.01), and trending smaller in middle frontal gyrus (t=1.6, df=41, p<0.06). FIGURE 4. Quantitative Results of Region of Interest Analysis for Percent Active Voxels for Patients and Comparison Subjects in Six Hypothesized Regions of InterestPatients showed lower overall extent of activation than comparison subjects (main effect of group). Activation for the UAD condition was lower than for the AAT condition (main effect of condition). ACG=anterior cingulate gyrus; IFG=inferior frontal gyrus; MFG=middle frontal gyrus; STG=superior temporal gyrus; BG=basal ganglia; TH=thalamusThis analysis was repeated using percent signal change as the dependent variable and results were generally consistent with the preceding analysis. An omnibus ANOVA using PSC as the dependent variable revealed main effects for group (F=5.46, df=1, 41, p<0.03), region (F=22.4, df=5, 205, p<0.0001), and condition (F=17.5, df=1, 41, p<0.0001), and an interaction of region×condition (F=2.5, df=5, 205, p<0.05); there was no main effect for hemisphere (F=1.4, df=1, 41, p=0.25) and no interaction effects for region×group (F=0.73, df=5, 205, p<0.6), condition×group (F=0.11, df=1, 41, p<0.75), hemisphere×group (F=0.91, df=1, 41, p=0.35) or region×condition×group (F=0.69, df=5, 205, p<0.63). Post hoc analyses showed greater overall intensity of activation in comparison subjects relative to patients and in the AAT condition relative to the UAD condition. Post hoc analysis showed lower AAT activation in the patient group in inferior frontal gyrus (t=2.1, df=41, p<0.05) and thalamus (t=2.3, df=41, p<0.02), and for the UAD condition in thalamus (t=2.7, df=41, p<0.01) and marginally lower in the left superior temporal gyrus (t=1.6, df=41, p=0.06).As the two groups differed in their performance accuracy in the AAT condition, we reanalyzed the data from this condition using only trials with correct responses (the UAD condition was not reanalyzed since no behavioral responses were involved). This analysis resulted in a marginal main effect for group (F=3.6, df=1, 41, p<0.07), a significant main effect for hemisphere (F=7.6, df=1, 41, p<0.01), and for region (F=6.7, df=5, 205, p<0.0001), as well as significant interaction effects for region×group (F=2.4, df=5, 205, p<0.04), region×hemisphere (F=7.1, df=5, 205, p<0.0001), and region×hemisphere×group (F=4.2, df=5, 205, p<0.005). These effects parallel those from the analysis of all trials. Post hoc analyses showed a smaller spatial extent of activation in patients than comparison subjects in the right inferior frontal gyrus (t=2.1, df=41, p<0.01), left basal ganglia (t=2.6, df=41, p<0.01), and the left anterior cingulate gyrus (t=1.8, df=41, p<0.05).A post hoc analysis of the middle frontal gyrus and superior temporal gyrus showed that AAT activation in the middle frontal gyrus was highly correlated with UAD activation in superior temporal gyrus in patients (percentage of activated voxels; r=0.70, p=0.0001), but not in comparison subjects (r=0.12, p=0.65).Effects of Clinical MeasuresThere was a negative correlation between illness duration and percentage of activated voxels for the AAT condition in the superior temporal gyrus (r=−0.51, p<0.01). Because age was highly correlated with illness duration (r=0.81, p<0.0001), we also examined whether superior temporal gyrus activation varied with age in comparison subjects; no effect was observed (r=0.08, p<0.77). More modest negative correlations were also found between illness duration and percentage of activated voxels to the AAT condition in the thalamus (r=−0.38, p<0.06) and inferior frontal gyrus (r=−0.35, p<0.08). For percentage signal change, only the superior temporal gyrus (r=−0.38, p<0.06) was significantly correlated with illness duration for the AAT condition. All these measures were uncorrelated with age in comparison subjects. Negative correlations between medication dose and percentage of activated voxels for the AAT condition were noted in the subcortical regions, basal ganglia (r=−0.42, p<0.04) and thalamus (r=−0.38, p=0.06). Activations in frontal and temporal regions were uncorrelated with medication dose.DISCUSSIONWe assessed functional brain differences with BOLD activation during automatic and controlled auditory information processing in participants with schizophrenia and age-matched comparison subjects. During unattended auditory processing, patients showed lower activation than comparison subjects in the middle frontal gyrus and thalamus as well as in the superior temporal gyrus. During controlled auditory processing, patients showed lower activation in the middle frontal gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate gyrus, insula, and subcortical regions.Functional Changes in Schizophrenia With Automatic Processing Our results demonstrated reduced activation in patients relative to comparison subjects during automatic auditory processing in the right middle frontal gyrus, thalamus, and superior temporal gyrus. This suggests that auditory impairment in schizophrenia extends beyond the superior temporal gyrus and Heschl’s gyrus as shown in earlier fMRI studies, 10 , 24 , 25 and as inferred from event-related potential studies of the auditory mismatch negativity. 26 Impaired thalamic function has been reported in controlled processing, 4 but not in automatic processing. Studies of comparison subjects show that automatic auditory processing occurs primarily at the level of the primary auditory cortex. 15 , 27 However, preattentive change detection involves further interaction between sensory and prefrontal cortices that can facilitate potential behavioral responses by engaging frontal networks in response to sensory inputs. 14 Evidence of a frontal contribution to mismatch negativity that deteriorates in schizophrenia is suggested by event-related potential findings, 28 , 29 but this has not been demonstrated with neuroimaging until now. Thus, our findings show that prefrontal dysfunction in schizophrenia is associated not only with higher cognitive functions, 4 , 11 but with sensory level auditory processing as well. It is possible that the middle frontal gyrus dysfunction we observed with automatic processing may underlie the clinically observed distractibility in schizophrenia and may reflect a porous mechanism for filtering sensory stimuli in noisy environments. 30 Evidence shows that the brain’s response to task-irrelevant sensory changes is strongly influenced by intermodal attentional demands. 31 We have previously published findings from healthy subjects performing a continuous perceptual-motor visual tracking task at two levels of difficulty while simultaneously hearing a series of task-irrelevant standard tone pips and infrequent pitch-deviant tones. Our fMRI results revealed that the unattended pitch-deviant tones strongly activated superior temporal and frontal cortical regions. These activations were significantly modulated by the tracking difficulty of the primary task, 32 but this effect is greater under high load conditions. In this respect, results from the patients in the current study are consistent with the literature describing healthy subjects. In the present study, the primary task is a low load visual discrimination task, perhaps especially so for the comparison group as evidenced by their superior behavioral performance. Thus, the low load visual task may not have been sufficiently demanding to unveil the effect in question in comparison subjects, but it was evident in patients for whom the task difficulty was greater. Functional Changes in Schizophrenia With Controlled Processing We observed impaired behavioral performance in patients with schizophrenia during controlled auditory processing, which, consistent with earlier work, was accompanied by lower activation in inferior frontal gyrus, middle frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate gyrus, subcortical structures (basal ganglia, thalamus), and insula. 4 , 6 , 8 Separate region of interest analyses for all trials and for correct trials revealed concordant findings. Patients had lower activation in the inferior frontal gyrus, basal ganglia, and thalamus for both analyses and lower activation in the anterior cingulate gyrus on correct trials. The unique anterior cingulate gyrus finding for the correct trials is plausible given its role in conflict monitoring and error detection 33 and given evidence of its dysfunction in schizophrenia. 34 Recent studies show that ventral limbic structures such as the amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, and rostral anterior cingulate gyrus are also impaired in schizophrenia during automatic auditory processing. 6 , 8 However, activation of these ventral structures could be attributed to the inclusion of a third novel tone (nontarget) that was not included in the present study. Though we found a relatively focal region of the anterior superior temporal gyrus that showed lower activation in the schizophrenia group in the random effects analysis, the region of interest analysis did not show such a difference. Previous studies are unclear about the superior temporal gyrus, with deficits noted in some studies, 4 , 8 but not others. 6Comparison of Automatic and Controlled Processing in Schizophrenia We found that controlled processing generally elicited greater activation relative to automatic processing in both patient and comparison groups. However, for controlled processing, the comparison group showed additional areas with greater activation as compared to the patient group. Despite diminished superior temporal gyrus activity in patients during unattended processing, a corresponding diminution was not detected for attended processing. We have considered two possible explanations for this difference. The first concerns the role of the visual task that was performed concurrently with
Referência(s)