Revisão Acesso aberto Revisado por pares

Sex Influences on the Brain: An Issue Whose Time Has Come

2015; Cell Press; Volume: 88; Issue: 6 Linguagem: Inglês

10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.021

ISSN

1097-4199

Autores

Larry Cahill, Dana W. Aswad,

Tópico(s)

Hormonal Regulation and Hypertension

Resumo

The issue of sex influences on the brain is rapidly moving center stage, driven by abundant results proving that subject sex can and regularly does alter, negate, and even reverse neuroscientific findings and conclusions down to the molecular level and thus can no longer be justifiably marginalized or ignored. The issue of sex influences on the brain is rapidly moving center stage, driven by abundant results proving that subject sex can and regularly does alter, negate, and even reverse neuroscientific findings and conclusions down to the molecular level and thus can no longer be justifiably marginalized or ignored. Early in 2013, the US Food and Drug Administration (FDA) ordered the makers of the sleep aid Ambien to cut their recommended dose in half—but only for women. In essence, the FDA was acknowledging that millions of women had been overdosing on the drug since its approval. In 2014, the Director of the National Institutes of Health, Francis Collins, and the Director of the NIH Office of Research on Women's Health, Janine Clayton, published a game-changing article stating that all NIH-funded research—for the first time ever—will be required to actively consider sex influences (Clayton and Collins, 2014Clayton J.A. Collins F.S. Nature. 2014; 509: 282-283Crossref PubMed Scopus (1084) Google Scholar). What lies behind these remarkable developments? The answer to this question is the rapidly burgeoning weight of evidence proving that sex matters in different kinds of ways, from the level of the intact human down to the level of ion-channel function, and everywhere in between. Numerous excellent reviews document this striking development (Cosgrove et al., 2007Cosgrove K.P. Mazure C.M. Staley J.K. Biol. Psychiatry. 2007; 62: 847-855Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (660) Google Scholar, Jazin and Cahill, 2010Jazin E. Cahill L. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010; 11: 9-17Crossref PubMed Scopus (172) Google Scholar, McCarthy et al., 2009McCarthy M.M. Auger A.P. Bale T.L. De Vries G.J. Dunn G.A. Forger N.G. Murray E.K. Nugent B.M. Schwarz J.M. Wilson M.E. J. Neurosci. 2009; 29: 12815-12823Crossref PubMed Scopus (325) Google Scholar). As a recent example, the laboratory of one of us (D.A.) made the serendipitous discovery that endogenous levels of phosphorylation of synapsin I (a major regulator of synaptic transmission throughout the CNS) differ dramatically between male and female mice (Qin et al., 2013Qin Z. Kaufman R.S. Khoury R.N. Khoury M.K. Aswad D.W. PLoS ONE. 2013; 8: e80758Crossref PubMed Scopus (11) Google Scholar). This was highly unexpected because (1) there is no difference between the sexes in total levels of either synapsin I or the kinase that phosphorylates it, and (2) the phosphorylation of synapsin in rodents has been investigated by several researchers over the past 36 years, but in each case, only one sex was used—either males (Strömbom et al., 1979Strömbom U. Forn J. Dolphin A.C. Greengard P. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 1979; 76: 4687-4690Crossref PubMed Scopus (28) Google Scholar, Yamagata et al., 1995Yamagata Y. Obata K. Greengard P. Czernik A.J. Neuroscience. 1995; 64: 1-4Crossref PubMed Scopus (35) Google Scholar) or females (Iwata et al., 1996Iwata S. Hewlett G.H. Ferrell S.T. Czernik A.J. Meiri K.F. Gnegy M.E. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 1996; 278: 1428-1434PubMed Google Scholar). There are many examples like this across the neuroscience spectrum. Sex influences on brain function have been reported regarding the neural/genetic underpinnings of addiction (Barker et al., 2010Barker J.M. Torregrossa M.M. Arnold A.P. Taylor J.R. J. Neurosci. 2010; 30: 9140-9144Crossref PubMed Scopus (96) Google Scholar), stress responses (Bangasser, 2013Bangasser D.A. Biol. Sex Differ. 2013; 4: 2Crossref PubMed Scopus (69) Google Scholar), genetic changes with human aging (Berchtold et al., 2008Berchtold N.C. Cribbs D.H. Coleman P.D. Rogers J. Head E. Kim R. Beach T. Miller C. Troncoso J. Trojanowski J.Q. et al.Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2008; 105: 15605-15610Crossref PubMed Scopus (405) Google Scholar), human brain connectivity (Ingalhalikar et al., 2014Ingalhalikar M. Smith A. Parker D. Satterthwaite T.D. Elliott M.A. Ruparel K. Hakonarson H. Gur R.E. Gur R.C. Verma R. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2014; 111: 823-828Crossref PubMed Scopus (670) Google Scholar), schizophrenia (Abazyan et al., 2014Abazyan B. Dziedzic J. Hua K. Abazyan S. Yang C. Mori S. Pletnikov M.V. Guilarte T.R. Schizophr Bull. 2014; 40: 575-584Crossref PubMed Scopus (42) Google Scholar), pre-natal nicotine exposure (Cao et al., 2013Cao J. Wang J. Dwyer J.B. Gautier N.M. Wang S. Leslie F.M. Li M.D. Transl Psychiatry. 2013; 3: e247Crossref PubMed Scopus (30) Google Scholar), drug responses (Reilly et al., 1990Reilly P.E. Thompson D.A. Mason S.R. Hooper W.D. Mol. Pharmacol. 1990; 37: 767-774PubMed Google Scholar), ischemia (Lang and McCullough, 2008Lang J.T. McCullough L.D. J. Transl. Med. 2008; 6: 33Crossref PubMed Scopus (152) Google Scholar), microcephaly (Rimol et al., 2010Rimol L.M. Agartz I. Djurovic S. Brown A.A. Roddey J.C. Kähler A.K. Mattingsdal M. Athanasiu L. Joyner A.H. Schork N.J. et al.Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging InitiativeProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010; 107: 384-388Crossref PubMed Scopus (98) Google Scholar), microglia function (Crain et al., 2013Crain J.M. Nikodemova M. Watters J.J. J. Neurosci. Res. 2013; 91: 1143-1151Crossref PubMed Scopus (252) Google Scholar), and pain perception (Sorge et al., 2015Sorge R.E. Mapplebeck J.C. Rosen S. Beggs S. Taves S. Alexander J.K. Martin L.J. Austin J.S. Sotocinal S.G. Chen D. et al.Nat. Neurosci. 2015; 18: 1081-1083Crossref PubMed Scopus (744) Google Scholar) to name only a tiny fraction of the extant findings. For a review of sex differences in molecular neuroscience, see Jazin and Cahill, 2010Jazin E. Cahill L. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010; 11: 9-17Crossref PubMed Scopus (172) Google Scholar. The immense power of carefully attending to potential sex effects, and conversely the confusion caused by assuming they do not exist, is nicely exemplified by the work of Rimol et al., 2010Rimol L.M. Agartz I. Djurovic S. Brown A.A. Roddey J.C. Kähler A.K. Mattingsdal M. Athanasiu L. Joyner A.H. Schork N.J. et al.Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging InitiativeProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010; 107: 384-388Crossref PubMed Scopus (98) Google Scholar concerning the genetic control of human brain size. Humans are distinguished by the dramatic and relatively recent evolutionary growth of their brains relative to body size. As one approach to understanding how this happened, Rimol et al., 2010Rimol L.M. Agartz I. Djurovic S. Brown A.A. Roddey J.C. Kähler A.K. Mattingsdal M. Athanasiu L. Joyner A.H. Schork N.J. et al.Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging InitiativeProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010; 107: 384-388Crossref PubMed Scopus (98) Google Scholar examined loss-of-function mutations in genes associated with congenital primary recessive microcephaly. They did so by correlating SNPs from four such genes with brain morphometry obtained with MRI. Critically, they note that previous attempts to link common variants in this gene family to variation in human brain morphology were both largely unsuccessful, and generally assumed that sex differences would not exist. However, Rimol et al., 2010Rimol L.M. Agartz I. Djurovic S. Brown A.A. Roddey J.C. Kähler A.K. Mattingsdal M. Athanasiu L. Joyner A.H. Schork N.J. et al.Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging InitiativeProc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010; 107: 384-388Crossref PubMed Scopus (98) Google Scholar detected sex-specific associations between the SNPs and brain morphometry in two separate cohorts: SNPs of the gene CDK5RAP2 significantly related to brain phenotype only in men, whereas SNPs of the genes MCPH1 and ASPM significantly related to brain phenotype only in women. They note that these findings "are unique in their demonstration of an association between common variants of any of the MCPH genes and brain structure in humans." Had they assumed that sex would not matter, they would almost certainly have missed this discovery. Another recent paper highlights the fact that sex influences not only brain function, but also interactions between the brain and other systems. Sorge et al., 2015Sorge R.E. Mapplebeck J.C. Rosen S. Beggs S. Taves S. Alexander J.K. Martin L.J. Austin J.S. Sotocinal S.G. Chen D. et al.Nat. Neurosci. 2015; 18: 1081-1083Crossref PubMed Scopus (744) Google Scholar examined interactions between neural and immune function in pain perception. Using multiple approaches, they found that, unlike what they and others had previously found using males, pain hypersensitivity in females does not involve a contribution from microglia. In contrast to male mice, female mice achieve similar levels of pain hypersensitivity using adaptive immune cells, likely T lymphocytes. These results provide yet more evidence that the neural mechanisms of pain differ substantially in males and females. Despite the evidence, Sorge et al., 2015Sorge R.E. Mapplebeck J.C. Rosen S. Beggs S. Taves S. Alexander J.K. Martin L.J. Austin J.S. Sotocinal S.G. Chen D. et al.Nat. Neurosci. 2015; 18: 1081-1083Crossref PubMed Scopus (744) Google Scholar note that male-only studies remain "standard" in the pain field, as in most of neuroscience, with results extrapolated freely to females. We fail to see how such an approach at this juncture is even scientifically defensible. The converging evidence of the influence of sex on brain function across the entire spectrum of brain science raises the question, "If sex matters so much to brain function at essentially all levels, how did we miss this fact for so long?" A complete answer is beyond the scope of this short review, but we offer two thoughts. First, and simplest, neuroscience to this day studies male animals almost exclusively (Beery and Zucker, 2011Beery A.K. Zucker I. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011; 35: 565-572Crossref PubMed Scopus (912) Google Scholar), meaning sex differences have no chance of ever being discovered, much less missed, in the vast majority of neuroscience studies. And second, even when sex differences are present, conceptual blinders can easily block them from view. An example concerns the original investigations of the well-known sleep aid Ambien (zolpidem), which clearly showed that the drug was being metabolized much more slowly in females than in males. Yet investigators at the time appear to have simply dismissed the result as being unimportant. It was not until some 20 years later—after the deaths of women killed while driving the morning after they took the drug, but while still unexpectedly under its influence—that the FDA finally mandated that the recommended dosage in women be cut in half. No one knows how many "Ambiens" are out there now or will be generated in the future if neuroscience clings to its very widespread, long-standing view of sex influences as mildly important at best, a nuisance at worst. Importantly, the National Institute of Health is finally recognizing the profound importance of exploring sex influences and recently announced a new policy stating in part that, starting with FY2016, research applications "will include accounting for sex as a biological variable in the Research Strategy section" and that "strong justification…must be provided for applications proposing to study one sex." Such justification cannot include the fact that little or no extant literature on a specific topic exists (see http://orwh.od.nih.gov/sexinscience/overview/pdf/NOT-OD-15-102_Guidance.pdf). A previous commentary on the issue of sex influences in this journal (Eliot, 2011Eliot L. Neuron. 2011; 72: 895-898Abstract Full Text Full Text PDF PubMed Scopus (61) Google Scholar) referred to "the trouble with sex differences." Eliot focused on the issue of sex difference findings being misconstrued by the general public and encouraged neuroscientists to help convey appropriate messages to the public. We agree, but add that neuroscientists cannot do this until they themselves understand what the essential right message is, namely that the issue of sex influences is clearly very important at all levels of brain function yet remains massively understudied. The "trouble" with sex differences at present in our view is the continuing reluctance of so many neuroscientists to pursue the issue despite the evidence. The French novelist Victor Hugo famously said, "There is one thing more powerful than all the armies in the world, and that is an idea whose time has come." We think that the idea that "sex matters" for brain science is an idea whose time has come. The health of both men and women now requires research scientists to recognize this fact and to start adjusting their science accordingly.

Referência(s)
Altmetric
PlumX