Editorial Revisado por pares

Experimental Psychology

2010; Hogrefe Verlag; Volume: 57; Issue: 1 Linguagem: Inglês

10.1027/1618-3169/a000001

ISSN

2190-5142

Autores

Edgar Erdfelder,

Tópico(s)

Cultural Differences and Values

Resumo

EditorialExperimental PsychologyA Note on Statistical AnalysisEdgar ErdfelderEdgar Erdfelder Universität Mannheim, Germany Published Online:February 22, 2010https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000001PDFView Full Text ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinkedInReddit SectionsMoreReferences American Psychological Association . (2010). Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (6th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Baayen, R. H. , Davidson, D. J. , Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412, doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Bar-Anan, Y. , Nosek, B. A. , Vianello, M. (2009). The sorting paired features task: A measure of association strengths. Experimental Psychology, 56, 329–343, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.5.329. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Bianchi, M. , Machunsky, M. , Steffens, M. C. , Mummendey, A. (2009). Like me or like us: Is ingroup projection just social projection? Experimental Psychology, 56, 198–205, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.3.198. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Bredenkamp, J. (1969). Über die Anwendung des Signifikanztests bei theorietestenden Experimenten [Application of significance tests in theory-testing experiments]. Psychologische Beiträge, 11, 275–285. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Bredenkamp, J. (1972). Der Signifikanztest in der psychologischen Forschung [The significance test in psychological research] Frankfurt/Main, Germany: Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 335–359. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Cumming, G. , Fidler, F. (2009). Confidence intervals: Better answers to better questions. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 217, 15–26, doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.217.1.15. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Curran-Everett, D. , & Benos, D. J. (2004). Guidelines for reporting statistics in journals published by the American Physiological Society. Advances in Physiology Education, 28, 85–87, doi: 10.1152/advan.00019.2004. First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google Scholar Deroost, N. , Coomans, D. , Soetens, E. (2009). Perceptual load improves the expression but not learning of relevant sequence information. Experimental Psychology, 56, 84–91, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.2.84. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Deutsch, R. , Kordts-Freudinger, R. , Gawronski, B. , Strack, F. (2009). Fast and fragile: A new look at the automaticity of negation processing. Experimental Psychology, 56, 434–446, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.6.434. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Duyck, W. , Warlop, N. (2009). Translation priming between the native language and a second language. Experimental Psychology, 56, 173–179, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.3.173. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Erdfelder, E. (1984). Zur Bedeutung und Kontrolle des beta-Fehlers bei der inferenzstatistischen Prüfung log-linearer Modelle [On significance and control of beta errors in statistical tests of log-linear models] Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 15, 18–32. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Erdfelder, E. , Auer, T. S. , Hilbig, B. E. , Aßfalg, A. , Moshagen, M. , Nadarevic, L. (2009). Multinomial processing tree models: A review of the literature. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 217, 108–124, doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.217.3.108. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Evans, J. S. B. T. , Handley, S. J. , Bacon, A. M. (2009). Reasoning under time pressure: A study of causal conditional inference. Experimental Psychology, 56, 188–197, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.2.77. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Faul, F. , Erdfelder, E. , Buchner, A. , Lang, A.-G. (2009). Statistical power analysis using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149–1160. doi:10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149. First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google Scholar Fidler, F. , Loftus, G. R. (2009). Why figures with error bars should replace p values: Some conceptual arguments and empirical demonstrations. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 217, 27–37, doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.217.1.27. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Jescheniak, J. D. , Oppermann, F. , Hantsch, A. , Wagner, V. , Mädebach, A. , Schriefers, H. (2009). Do perceived context pictures automatically activate their phonological code?. Experimental Psychology, 56, 56–65, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.1.56. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Johansson, T. (2009). Strengthening the case for stimulus-specificity in artificial grammar learning: No evidence for abstract representations with extended exposure. Experimental Psychology, 56, 188–197, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.3.188. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Kaufmann, M. , Betsch, T. (2009). Origins of the sample size effect in explicit evaluative judgment. Experimental Psychology, 56, 344–353, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.5.344. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Koele, P. (1982). Calculating power in analysis of variance. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 513–516. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Lemhöfer, K. , Radach, R. (2009). Task context effects in bilingual nonword processing. Experimental Psychology, 56, 41–47, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.1.41. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Locker, L. , Hoffman, L. , Bovaird, J. A. (2007). On the use of multilevel modeling as an alternative to items analysis in psycholinguistic research. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 723–730. First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google Scholar Machunsky, M. , Meiser, T. , & Mummendey, A. (2009). On the crucial role of the mental ingroup representation for ingroup bias and the ingroup prototypicality-ingroup bias link. Experimental Psychology, 56, 156–164, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.3.156. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Marewski, J. N. , & Olsson, H. (2009). Beyond the null ritual: Formal modeling of psychological processes. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 217, 49–60, doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.217.1.49. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Martin, D. , Nind, L. K. , Macrae, C. N. (2009). Lateralized repetition priming for unfamiliar faces. Experimental Psychology, 56, 147–155, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.3.147. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Melton, A. W. (1962). Editorial. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 553–557. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Öllinger, M. , Jones, G. , Knoblich, G. (2008). Investigating the effect of mental set on insight problem solving. Experimental Psychology, 55, 269–282, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.55.4.269. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Perea, M. , Dunabeitia, J. A. , Carreiras, M. (2008). Transposed-letter priming effects for close versus distant transpositions. Experimental Psychology, 55, 384–393, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.55.6.384. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Raaijmakers, J. G. W. (2003). A further look at "the language-as-fixed-effect fallacy". Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 141–151. First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google Scholar Raaijmakers, J. G. W. , Schrijnemakers, J. M. C. , Gremmen, F. (1999). How to deal with "the language-as-fixed-effect fallacy": Common misconceptions and alternative solutions. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 416–426. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Rosnow, R. L. , & Rosenthal, R. (2009). Effect sizes: Why, when, and how to use them. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 217, 6–14, doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.217.1.6. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Rouder, J. N. , Speckman, P. L. , Sun, D. C. , Morey, R. D. , Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 225–237, doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.225. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Stahl, C. , Meiser, T. (2009). New directions in multinomial modeling. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 217, 105–107, doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.217.3.105. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Sugrue, K. , Strange, D. , Hayne, H. (2009). False memories in the DRM paradigm: Age-related differences in lure activation and source monitoring. Experimental Psychology, 56, 354–360, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.5.354. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Vainio, S. , Hyönä, J. , Pajunen, A. (2009). Lexical predictability exerts robust effects on fixation duration, but not on initial landing position during reading. Experimental Psychology, 56, 66–74, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.56.1.66. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Welvaert, M. , Farioli, F. , Grainger, J. (2008). Graded effects of number of inserted letters in superset priming. Experimental Psychology, 55, 54–63, doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.55.1.54. First citation in articleLink, Google Scholar Westermann, R. , & Hager, W. (1986). Error probabilities in educational and psychological research. Journal of Educational Statistics, 11, 117–146. First citation in articleCrossref, Google Scholar Wetzels, R. , Raaijmakers, J. G. W. , Jakab, E. , Wagenmakers, E. J. (2009). How to quantify support for and against the null hypothesis: A flexible WinBUGS implementation of a default Bayesian t test. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 225–237, doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.4.752. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar Wilkinson, L. , The Task Force on Statistical Inference (1999). Statistical methods in psychology journals: Guidelines and explanations. American Psychologist, 54, 594–604. First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarFiguresReferencesRelatedDetailsCited byDetecting Evidential Value and p-Hacking With the p-Curve Tool A Word of CautionEdgar Erdfelder and Daniel W. Heck20 December 2019 | Zeitschrift für Psychologie, Vol. 227, No. 4Minimizing Mistakes in Psychological Science30 January 2019 | Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, Vol. 2, No. 1Beyond inference by eye: Statistical and graphing practices in JEAB , 1992‐201712 February 2019 | Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, Vol. 111, No. 2Experimental Psychology Methodological Rigor and TransparencyChristoph Stahl21 February 2017 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 64, No. 1Cross-linguistic structural priming in multilinguals: Further evidence for shared syntaxJournal of Memory and Language, Vol. 90Wider die "Sternchenkunde"!21 August 2015 | Sportwissenschaft, Vol. 46, No. 2The Interplay between Subjectivity, Statistical Practice, and Psychological Science11 May 2016 | Collabra, Vol. 2, No. 1Second-order motor planning in children: insights from a cup-manipulation-task17 July 2014 | Psychological Research, Vol. 79, No. 4Experimental Psychology Toward Reproducible ResearchChristoph Stahl21 January 2014 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 61, No. 1Experimental Psychology A Case for Methodological Rigor and Theoretical RelevanceThorsten Meiser22 January 2013 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 60, No. 1Result-Blind Peer Reviews and Editorial Decisions A Missing Pillar of Scientific CultureWerner Greve, Arndt Bröder, and Edgar Erdfelder1 January 2013 | European Psychologist, Vol. 18, No. 4Effects of Age-of-Acquisition in the Word-Fragment Completion Task Evidence for an Orthographic Locus in Implicit MemoryPietro Spataro, Neil W. Mulligan, Emiddia Longobardi, and Clelia Rossi-Arnaud1 January 2012 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 59, No. 1Time in action contexts: learning when an action effect occurs17 May 2011 | Psychological Research, Vol. 76, No. 3Executive control is shared between sentence processing and digit maintenance: Evidence from a strictly timed dual-task paradigmJournal of Cognitive Psychology, Vol. 23, No. 7Experimental Psychology A Place for Innovative Research and Methodological DevelopmentsThorsten Meiser6 January 2011 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 58, No. 1Statistical Evidence in Experimental Psychology18 May 2011 | Perspectives on Psychological Science, Vol. 6, No. 3Much Pain, Little Gain? Paradigm-Specific Models and Methods in Experimental Psychology24 March 2011 | Perspectives on Psychological Science, Vol. 6, No. 2A heuristic mathematical approach for modeling constraint cumulativity: Contrastive focus in Spanish and CatalanThe Linguistic Review, Vol. 28, No. 2 Volume 57Issue 1October 2010ISSN: 1618-3169eISSN: 2190-5142 InformationExperimental Psychology (2010), 57, pp. 1-4 https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000001.© 2010Hogrefe PublishingPDF download

Referência(s)