Artigo Acesso aberto Revisado por pares

The perception of odor pleasantness is shared across cultures

2022; Elsevier BV; Volume: 32; Issue: 9 Linguagem: Inglês

10.1016/j.cub.2022.02.062

ISSN

1879-0445

Autores

Artin Arshamian, Richard C. Gerkin, Nicole Kruspe, Ewelina Wnuk, Simeon Floyd, Carolyn O’Meara, Gabriela Garrido Rodriguez, Johan N. Lundström, Joel D. Mainland, Asifa Majid,

Tópico(s)

Biochemical Analysis and Sensing Techniques

Resumo

Humans share sensory systems with a common anatomical blueprint, but individual sensory experience nevertheless varies. In olfaction, it is not known to what degree sensory perception, particularly the perception of odor pleasantness, is founded on universal principles,1Khan R.M. Luk C.-H. Flinker A. Aggarwal A. Lapid H. Haddad R. Sobel N. Predicting odor pleasantness from odorant structure: pleasantness as a reflection of the physical world.J. Neurosci. 2007; 27: 10015-10023Crossref PubMed Scopus (253) Google Scholar, 2Haddad R. Medhanie A. Roth Y. Harel D. Sobel N. Predicting odor pleasantness with an electronic nose.PLoS Comp. Biol. 2010; 6: e1000740Crossref PubMed Scopus (49) Google Scholar, 3Keller A. Gerkin R.C. Guan Y. Dhurandhar A. Turu G. Szalai B. Mainland J.D. Ihara Y. Yu C.W. Wolfinger R. et al.Predicting human olfactory perception from chemical features of odor molecules.Science. 2017; 355: 820-826Crossref PubMed Scopus (115) Google Scholar, 4Joussain P. Chakirian A. Kermen F. Rouby C. Bensafi M. Physicochemical influence on odor hedonics: where does it occur first?.Commun. Integr. Biol. 2011; 4: 563-565Crossref PubMed Google Scholar, 5Kermen F. Chakirian A. Sezille C. Joussain P. Le Goff G. Ziessel A. Chastrette M. Mandairon N. Didier A. Rouby C. Bensafi M. Molecular complexity determines the number of olfactory notes and the pleasantness of smells.Sci. Rep. 2011; 1: 206Crossref PubMed Scopus (76) Google Scholar dictated by culture,6Ayabe-Kanamura S. Schicker I. Laska M. Hudson R. Distel H. Kobayakawa T. Saito S. Differences in perception of everyday odors: a Japanese-German cross-cultural study.Chem. Senses. 1998; 23: 31-38Crossref PubMed Scopus (216) Google Scholar, 7Classen C. Howes D. Synnott A. Aroma: The Cultural History of Smell. Routledge, 1994Google Scholar, 8Distel H. Ayabe-Kanamura S. Martínez-Gómez M. Schicker I. Kobayakawa T. Saito S. Hudson R. Perception of everyday odors—correlation between intensity, familiarity and strength of hedonic judgement.Chem. Senses. 1999; 24: 191-199Crossref PubMed Scopus (195) Google Scholar, 9Maier-Nöth A. Schaal B. Leathwood P. Issanchou S. The lasting influences of early food-related variety experience: a longitudinal study of vegetable acceptance from 5 months to 6 years in two populations.PLoS One. 2016; 11: e0151356Crossref PubMed Scopus (62) Google Scholar, 10Pangborn R.M. Guinard J.-X. Davis R.G. Regional aroma preferences.Food Qual. Preference. 1988; 1: 11-19Crossref Scopus (48) Google Scholar, 11Poncelet J. Rinck F. Bourgeat F. Schaal B. Rouby C. Bensafi M. Hummel T. The effect of early experience on odor perception in humans: psychological and physiological correlates.Behav. Brain Res. 2010; 208: 458-465Crossref PubMed Scopus (38) Google Scholar, 12Wysocki C.J. Pierce J.D. Gilbert A.N. Geographic, cross-cultural, and individual variation in human olfaction.in: Smell and Taste in Health and Disease. Raven Press, 1991: 287-314Google Scholar, 13Yamin-Pasternak S. Kliskey A. Alessa L. Pasternak I. Schweitzer P. The rotten renaissance in the Bering Strait: loving, loathing, and washing the smell of foods with a (re)acquired taste.Curr. Anthropol. 2014; 55: 619-646Crossref Scopus (32) Google Scholar or merely a matter of personal taste.6Ayabe-Kanamura S. Schicker I. Laska M. Hudson R. Distel H. Kobayakawa T. Saito S. Differences in perception of everyday odors: a Japanese-German cross-cultural study.Chem. Senses. 1998; 23: 31-38Crossref PubMed Scopus (216) Google Scholar,8Distel H. Ayabe-Kanamura S. Martínez-Gómez M. Schicker I. Kobayakawa T. Saito S. Hudson R. Perception of everyday odors—correlation between intensity, familiarity and strength of hedonic judgement.Chem. Senses. 1999; 24: 191-199Crossref PubMed Scopus (195) Google Scholar, 9Maier-Nöth A. Schaal B. Leathwood P. Issanchou S. The lasting influences of early food-related variety experience: a longitudinal study of vegetable acceptance from 5 months to 6 years in two populations.PLoS One. 2016; 11: e0151356Crossref PubMed Scopus (62) Google Scholar, 10Pangborn R.M. Guinard J.-X. Davis R.G. Regional aroma preferences.Food Qual. Preference. 1988; 1: 11-19Crossref Scopus (48) Google Scholar,12Wysocki C.J. Pierce J.D. Gilbert A.N. Geographic, cross-cultural, and individual variation in human olfaction.in: Smell and Taste in Health and Disease. Raven Press, 1991: 287-314Google Scholar,14Logue A.W. Logue C.M. Uzzo R.G. McCarty M.J. Smith M.E. Food preferences in families.Appetite. 1988; 10: 169-180Crossref PubMed Scopus (58) Google Scholar To address this, we asked 225 individuals from 9 diverse nonwestern cultures—hunter-gatherer to urban dwelling—to rank the monomolecular odorants from most to least pleasant. Contrary to expectations, culture explained only 6% of the variance in pleasantness rankings, whereas individual variability or personal taste explained 54%. Importantly, there was substantial global consistency, with molecular identity explaining 41% of the variance in odor pleasantness rankings. Critically, these universal rankings were predicted by the physicochemical properties of out-of-sample molecules and out-of-sample pleasantness ratings given by a tenth group of western urban participants. Taken together, this shows human olfactory perception is strongly constrained by universal principles.

Referência(s)