Artigo Revisado por pares

Loss of Recent Memory After Bilateral Hippocampal Lesions

2000; American Psychiatric Association Publishing; Volume: 12; Issue: 1 Linguagem: Inglês

10.1176/jnp.12.1.103-a

ISSN

1545-7222

Autores

William Beecher Scoville, Brenda Milner,

Tópico(s)

Neuroscience and Neuropharmacology Research

Resumo

Back to table of contents Previous article Next article Classic ArticlesFull AccessLoss of Recent Memory After Bilateral Hippocampal LesionsWilliam Beecher Scoville, and Brenda MilnerWilliam Beecher ScovilleSearch for more papers by this author, and Brenda MilnerSearch for more papers by this authorPublished Online:1 Feb 2000https://doi.org/10.1176/jnp.12.1.103-aAboutSectionsPDF/EPUB ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinked InEmail In 1954 Scoville14 described a grave loss of recent memory which he had observed as a sequel to bilateral medial temporal lobe resection in one psychotic patient and one patient with intractable seizures. In both cases the operations had been radical ones, undertaken only when more conservative forms of treatment had failed. The removals extended posteriorly along the mesial surface of the temporal lobes for a distance of approximately 8 cm from the temporal tips and probably destroyed the anterior two-thirds of the hippocampus and hippocampal gyrus bilaterally, as well as the uncus and amygdala. The unexpected and persistent memory deficit which resulted seemed to us to merit further investigation. We have therefore carried out formal memory and intelligence testing of these two patients and also of eight other patients who had undergone similar, but less radical, bilateral medial temporal lobe resections. The present paper gives the results of these studies which point to the importance of the hippocampal complex for normal memory function. Whenever the hippocampus and hippocampal gyrus were damaged bilaterally in these operations some memory deficit was found, but not otherwise. We have chosen to report these findings in full, partly for their theoretical significance, and partly as a warning to others of the risk to memory involved in bilateral surgical lesions of the hippocampal region.OPERATIONSDuring the past seven years in an effort to preserve the overall personality in psychosurgery some 300 fractional lobotomies have been performed, largely on seriously ill schizophrenic patients who had failed to respond to other forms of treatment. The aim in these fractional procedures was to secure as far as possible any beneficial effects a complete frontal lobotomy might have, while at the same time avoiding its undesirable side effects. And it was in fact found that undercutting limited to the orbital surfaces of both frontal lobes has an appreciable therapeutic effect in psychosis and yet does not cause any new personality deficit to appear (Scoville, Wilk, and Pepe, 195116). In view of the known close relationship between the posterior orbital and mesial temporal cortices (MacLean, 19529; Pribram and Kruger, 195413), it was hoped that still greater psychiatric benefit might be obtained by extending the orbital undercutting so as to destroy parts of the mesial temporal cortex bilaterally. Accordingly, in 30 severely deteriorated cases, such partial temporal lobe resections were carried out, either with or without orbital undercutting. The surgical procedure has been described elsewhere (Scoville, Dunsmore, Liberson, Henry, and Pepe, 195315) and is illustrated anatomically in Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3, and Figure 4. All the removals have been bilateral, extending for varying distances along the mesial surface of the temporal lobes. Five were limited to the uncus and underlying amygdaloid nucleus; all others encroached also upon the anterior hippocampus, the excisions being carried back 5 cm or more after bisecting the tips of the temporal lobes, with the temporal horn constituting the lateral edge of resection. In one case only in this psychotic group all tissue mesial to the temporal horns for a distance of at least 8 cm posterior to the temporal tips was destroyed, a removal which presumably included the anterior two-thirds of the hippocampal complex bilaterally.An equally radical bilateral medical temporal lobe resection was carried out in one young man (H. M.) with a long history of major and minor seizures uncontrollable by maximum medication of various forms, and showing diffuse electroencephalographic abnormality. This frankly experimental operation was considered justifiable because the patient was totally incapacitated by his seizures and these had proven refractory to a medical approach. It was suggested because of the known epileptogenic qualities of the uncus and hippocampal complex and because of the relative absence of postoperative seizures in our temporal lobe resections as compared with fractional lobotomies in other areas. The operation was carried out with the understanding and approval of the patient and his family, in the hope of lessening his seizures to some extent. At operation the medial surfaces of both temporal lobes were exposed and recordings were taken from both surface and depth electrodes before any tissue was removed; but again no discrete epileptogenic focus was found. Bilateral resection was then carried out, extending posteriorly for a distance of 8 cm from the temporal tips.RESULTSThe psychiatric findings bearing upon the treatment of schizophrenia have already been reported (Scoville and others, 195315). Briefly, it was found that bilateral resections limited to the medial portions of the temporal lobes were without significant therapeutic effect in psychosis, although individual patients (including the one with the most radical removal) did in fact show some improvement. There have been no gross changes in personality. This is particularly clear in the case of the epileptic, nonpsychotic patient whose present cheerful placidity does not differ appreciably from his preoperative status and who, in the opinion of his family, has shown no personality change. Neurological changes in the group have also been minimal. The incidence of severity of seizures in the epileptic patient were sharply reduced for the first year after operation, and although he is once again having both major and minor attacks, these attacks no longer leave him stuporous, as they formerly did. It has therefore been possible to reduce his medication considerably. As far as general intelligence is concerned, the epileptic patient has actually improved slightly since operation, possibly because he is less drowsy than before. The psychotic patients were for the most part too disturbed before operation for finer testing of higher mental functions to be carried out, but certainly there is no indication of any general intellectual impairment resulting from the operation in those patients for whom the appropriate test data are available.There has been one striking and totally unexpected behavioral result: a grave loss of recent memory in those cases in which the medial temporal lobe resection was so extensive as to involve the major portion of the hippocampal complex bilaterally. The psychotic patient having the most radical excision (extending 8 cm from the tips of the temporal lobes bilaterally) has shown a profound postoperative memory disturbance, but unfortunately this was not recognized at the time because of her disturbed emotional state. In the nonpsychotic patient the loss was immediately apparent. After operation this young man could no longer recognize the hospital staff nor find his way to the bathroom, and he seemed to recall nothing of the day-to-day events of his hospital life. There was also a partial retrograde amnesia, inasmuch as he did not remember the death of a favorite uncle three years previously, nor anything of the period in hospital, yet could recall some trivial events that had occurred just before his admission to the hospital. His early memories were apparently vivid and intact.This patient's memory defect has persisted without improvement to the present time, and numerous illustrations of its severity could be given. Ten months ago the family moved from their old house to a new one a few blocks away on the same street; he still has not learned the new address, though remembering the old one perfectly, nor can he be trusted to find his way home alone. Moreover, he does not know where objects in continual use are kept; for example, his mother still has to tell him where to find the lawn mower, even though he may have been using it only the day before. She also states that he will do the same jigsaw puzzles day after day without showing any practice effect and that he will read the same magazines over and over again without finding their contents familiar. This patient has even eaten luncheon in front of one of us (B. M.) without being able to name, a mere half-hour later, a single item of food he had eaten; in fact, he could not remember having eaten luncheon at all. Yet to a casual observer this man seems like a relatively normal individual, since his understanding and reasoning are undiminished.The discovery of severe memory defect in these two patients led us to study further all patients in the temporal lobe series who were sufficiently cooperative to permit formal psychological testing. The operation sample included, in addition to the two radical resections, one bilateral removal of the uncus, extending 4 cm posterior to the temporal tips, and six bilateral medial temporal lobe resections in which the removal was carried back 5 or 6 cm to include also a portion of the anterior hippocampus; in three of these six cases the temporal lobe resection was combined with orbital undercutting. One unilateral case was also studied in which right inferior temporal lobectomy and hippocampectomy had been carried out for the relief of incisural herniation due to malignant edema (Figure 5). We found some memory impairment in all the bilateral cases in which the removal was carried far enough posteriorly to damage the hippocampus and hippocampal gyrus, but in only one of these six additional cases (D. C.) did the memory loss equal in severity that seen in the two most radical excisions. The case with bilateral excision of the uncus (in which the removal can have involved only the amygdaloid and peri-amygdaloid areas) showed excellent memory function. The unilateral operation, extensive as it was, has caused no lasting memory impairment, though some disturbance of recent memory was noted in the early postoperative period (Scoville, 195414); we now attribute this deficit to temporary interference with the functioning of the hippocampal zone of the opposite hemisphere by contralateral pressure.The histories and individual test results for these 10 cases are reported below, and Table 1 summarizes the principal findings. For purposes of comparison the cases have been divided into three groups representing different degrees of memory impairment.GROUP I: SEVERE MEMORY DEFECTIn this category are those patients who since operation appear to forget the incidents of their daily life as fast as they occur. It is interesting that all these patients were able to retain a three-figure number or a pair of unrelated words for several minutes, if care was taken not to distract them in the interval. However, they forgot the instant attention was diverted to a new topic. Since in normal life the focus of attention is constantly changing, such individuals show an apparently complete anterograde amnesia. This severe defect was observed in the two patients having the most radical bilateral medial temporal lobe excisions (with the posterior limit of removal approximately 8 cm from the temporal tips) and in one other case, a bilateral 5.5 cm medial temporal excision. These three cases will now be described.Case 1, H. M. This 29-year-old motor winder, a high school graduate, had had minor seizures since the age of 10 and major seizures since the age of 16. The small attacks lasted about 40 seconds, during which he would be unresponsive, opening his mouth, closing his eyes, and crossing both arms and legs; but he believed that he could “half hear what was going on.” The major seizures occurred without warning and with no lateralizing sign. They were generalized convulsions, with tongue-biting, urinary incontinence, and loss of consciousness followed by prolonged somnolence. Despite heavy and varied anticonvulsant medication the major attacks had increased in frequency and severity through the years until the patient was quite unable to work.The etiology of this patient's attacks is not clear. He was knocked down by a bicycle at the age of 9 and was unconscious for five minutes afterwards, sustaining a laceration of the left supra-orbital region. Later radiological studies, however, including two pneumo-encephalograms, have been completely normal, and the physical examination has always been negative.Electroencephalographic studies have consistently failed to show any localized epileptogenic area. In the examination of August 17, 1953, Dr. T. W. Liberson described diffuse slow activity with a dominant frequency of 6 to 8 per second. A short clinical attack was said to be accompanied by generalized 2 to 3 per second spike-and-wave discharge with a slight asymmetry in the central leads (flattening on the left).Despite the absence of any localizing sign, operation was considered justifiable for the reasons given above. On September 1, 1953, bilateral medial temporal lobe resection was carried out, extending posteriorly for a distance of 8 cm from the midpoints of the tips of the temporal lobes, with the temporal horns constituting the lateral edges of resection.After operation the patient was drowsy for a few days, but his subsequent recovery was uneventful apart from the grave memory loss already described. There has been no neurological deficit. An electroencephalogram taken one year after operation showed increased spike-and-wave activity which was maximal over the frontal areas and bilaterally synchronous. He continues to have seizures, but these are less incapacitating than before.Psychological Examination. This was performed on April 26, 1955. The memory defect was immediately apparent. The patient gave the date as March, 1953, and his age as 27. Just before coming into the examining room he had been talking to Dr. Karl Pribram, yet he had no recollection of this at all and denied that anyone had spoken to him. In conversation, he reverted constantly to boyhood events and seemed scarcely to realize that he had had an operation.On formal testing the contrast between his good general intelligence and his defective memory was most striking. On the Wechsler-Bellevue Intelligence Scale he achieved a full-scale IQ rating of 112, which compares favorably with the preoperative rating of 104 reported by Dr. Liselotte Fischer in August, 1953, the improvement in arithmetic being particularly striking. An extensive test battery failed to reveal any deficits in perception, abstract thinking, or reasoning ability, and his motivation remained excellent throughout.On the Wechsler Memory Scale (Wechsler, 194519) his immediate recall of stories and drawings fell far below the average level and on the “associate learning” subtest of this scale he obtained zero scores for the hard word associations, low scores for the easy associations, and failed to improve with repeated practice. These findings are reflected in the low memory quotient of 67. Moreover, on all tests we found that once he had turned to a new task the nature of the preceding one could no longer be recalled, nor the test recognized if repeated.In summary, this patient appears to have a complete loss of memory for events subsequent to bilateral medial temporal lobe resection 19 months before, together with a partial retrograde amnesia for the three years leading up to his operation; but early memories are seemingly normal and there is no impairment of personality or general intelligence.Case 2, D. C. This 47-year-old doctor was a paranoid schizophrenic with a four-year history of violent, combative behavior. Before his illness he had been practicing medicine in Chicago, but he had always shown paranoid trends and for this reason had had difficulty completing his medical training. His breakdown followed the loss of a lawsuit in 1950, at which time he made a homicidal attack on his wife which led to his admission to hospital. Since then both insulin and electroshock therapy had been tried without benefit and the prognosis was considered extremely poor. On May 13, 1954, at the request of Dr. Frederick Gibbs and Dr. John Kendrick, a bilateral medial temporal lobe resection combined with orbital undercutting was carried out at Manteno State Hospital (W. B. S., with the assistance of Dr. John Kendrick). The posterior limit of the removal was 5 cm from the sphenoid ridge, or roughly 5.5 cm from the tips of the temporal lobes, with the inferior horns of the ventricles forming the lateral edges of resection. Recording from depth electrodes at the time of operation showed spiking from the medial temporal regions bilaterally with some spread to the orbital surfaces of both frontal lobes, but after the removal had been completed a normal electroencephalographic record was obtained from the borders of the excision.Postoperative recovery was uneventful and there has been no neurological deficit. Since operation the patient has been outwardly friendly and tractable with no return of his former aggressive behavior, although the paranoid thought content persists; he is considered markedly improved. But he too shows a profound memory disturbance. At Manteno State Hospital he was described as “confused,” because since the operation he had been unable to find his way to bed and seemed no longer to recognize the hospital staff. However, no psychological examination was made there, and on November 29, 1955, he was transferred to Galesburg State Research Institute where he was interviewed by one of us (B. M.) on January 12, 1956.Psychological Findings. This patient presented exactly the same pattern of memory loss as H. M. He was courteous and cooperative throughout the examination, and the full-scale Wechsler IQ rating of 122 showed him to be still of superior intellect. Yet he had no idea where he was, explaining that naturally the surroundings were quite unfamiliar because he had only arrived there for the first time the night before. (In fact, he had been there six weeks.) He was unable to learn either the name of the hospital or the name of the examiner, despite being told them repeatedly. Each time he received the information as something new, and a moment later would deny having heard it. At the examiner's request he drew a dog and an elephant, yet half an hour later did not even recognize them as his own drawings. On the formal tests of the Wechsler Memory Scale his immediate recall of stories and drawings was poor, and the memory quotient of 70 is in sharp contrast to the IQ level. As with H. M., once a new task was introduced there was total amnesia for the preceding one; in his own words, the change of topic confused him. This man did not know that he had had a brain operation and did not recall being at Manteno State Hospital, although he had spent six months there before the operation as well as six months postoperatively. Yet he could give minute details of his early life and medical training (accurately, as far as we could tell).Case 3, M. B. This 55-year-old manic depressive woman, a former clerical worker, was admitted to Connecticut State Hospital on December 27, 1951, at which time she was described as anxious, irritable, argumentative, and restless, but well-orientated in all spheres. Her recent memory was normal, in that she knew how long she had been living in Connecticut and could give the date of her hospital admission and the exact times of various clinic appointments. On December 18, 1952, a radical bilateral medial temporal lobe resection was carried out, with the posterior limit of removal 8 cm from the temporal tips. Postoperatively she was stuporous and confused for one week, but then recovered rapidly and without neurological deficit. She has become neater and more even-tempered and is held to be greatly improved. However, psychological testing by Mr. I. Borganz in November, 1953, revealed a grave impairment of recent memory; she gave the year as 1950 and appeared to recall nothing of the events of the last three years. Yet her verbal intelligence proved to be normal.She was examined briefly by B. M. in April, 1955, at which time she showed a global loss of recent memory similar to that of H. M. and D. C. She had been brought to the examining room from another building, but had already forgotten this; nor could she describe any other part of the hospital although she had been living there continuously for nearly three and a half years. On the Wechsler Memory Scale her immediate recall of stories and drawings was inaccurate and fragmentary, and delayed recall was impossible for her even with prompting; when the material was presented again she failed to recognize it. Her conversation centered around her early life and she was unable to give any information about the years of her hospital stay. Vocabulary, attention span, and comprehension were normal, thus confirming Mr. Borganz' findings.GROUP II: MODERATELY SEVERE MEMORY DEFECTIn this second category are those patients who can be shown to retain some impression of new places and events, although they are unable to learn such arbitrary new associations as people's names and cannot be depended upon to carry out commissions. Subjectively, these patients complain of memory difficulty, and objectively, on formal tests, they do very poorly irrespective of the type of material to be memorized. The five remaining patients with bilateral medial temporal lobe removals extending 5 or 6 cm posteriorly from the temporal tips make up this group. Only two of these patients were well enough to permit thorough testing, but in all five cases enough data were obtained to establish that the patient did have a memory defect and that it was not of the gross type seen in Group I. The individual cases are reported below.Case 4, A. Z. This 35-year-old woman, a paranoid schizophrenic, had been in Connecticut State Hospital for three years and extensive electroshock therapy had been tried without lasting benefit. She was described as tense, assaultative, and sexually preocccupied. On November 29, 1951, bilateral medial temporal lobe resection was carried out under local anesthesia, the posterior limit of the removal being approximately 5 cm from the tips of the temporal lobes. During subpial resection of the right hippocampal cortex the surgeon inadvertently went through the arachnoid and injured by suction a portion of the right peduncle, geniculate, or hypothalamic region with immediate development of deep coma. The injury was visualized by extra-arachnoid inspection. Postoperatively the patient remained in stupor for 72 hours and exhibited a left spastic hemiplegia, contracted fixed pupils, strabismus, and lateral nystagmus of the right eye; vital signs remained constant and within normal limits. She slowly recovered the use of the left arm and leg and her lethargy gradually disappeared. By the seventh postoperative day she could walk without support and pupillary responses had returned to normal. The only residual neurological deficit has been a left homonymous hemianopia. Of particular interest was the dramatic postoperative improvement in her psychotic state with an early complete remission of her delusions, anxiety, and paranoid behavior. At the same time she showed a retrograde amnesia for the entire period of her illness.This patient was discharged from the hospital nine months after operation and is now able to earn her living as a domestic worker. However, she complains that her memory is poor, and psychological examination (April 27, 1995) three and a half years postoperatively confirms this. But the deficit is less striking than in the three cases reported above. This patient, for example, was able to give the address of the house where she worked although she had been there only two days, and she could even describe the furnishings in some detail although she had not yet learned the name of her employer. She was also able to give an accurate, though sketchy, description of a doctor who had spoken to her briefly that morning and whom she had never seen before. However, she could recall very little of the conversation.Formal testing at this time showed her intelligence to lie within the average range with no impairment of attention or concentration. The Wechsler-Bellevue IQ rating was 96. On the Wechsler Memory Scale her immediate recall of stories was normal, but passing from one story to the next was enough to make her unable to recall the first one, though a few fragments could be recovered with judicious prompting. She showed the same rapid forgetting on the “visual retention” subtest, indicating that the memory impairment was not specific to verbal material. Finally, she was conspicuously unsuccessful on the “associate learning” subtest, failing to master a single unfamiliar word association. This examination as a whole provides clear evidence of an impairment of recent memory.Case 5, M. R. This 40-year-old woman, a paranoid schizophrenic with superimposed alcoholism, had been a patient at Norwich State Hospital for 11 years, receiving extensive electroshock therapy. Bilateral medial temporal lobe resection combined with orbital undercutting was carried out on January 17, 1952, the posterior extent of removal being roughly 5 cm from the temporal tips. The patient has shown complete remission of psychotic symptoms and was discharged from the hospital on September 16, 1954, to the care of her family.Psychological Examination. This was performed on April 29, 1955. Tests showed this woman to be of superior intelligence, with a full-scale IQ of rating of 123 on the Wechsler Scale. However, she complained of poor memory, adding that she could remember faces and “the things that are important,” but that, to her great embarrassment, she forgot many ordinary daily happenings. Upon questioning she gave the year correctly but did not know the month or the day. She knew that she had had an operation in 1952 but did not recognize the surgeon (W. B. S.) nor recall his name. Formal testing revealed the same pattern of memory disturbance as A. Z. had shown, and the memory quotient of 81 compares most unfavorably with the high IQ rating. In conversation, she reverted constantly to discussion of her work during the years of depression and showed little knowledge of recent events.Case 6, A. R. This 38-year-old woman had been in hospital for five years with a diagnosis of hebephrenic schizophrenia. Before operation she was said to be noisy, combative, and suspicious, and electroshock therapy had caused only transient improvement in this condition. On May 31, 1951, bilateral medial temporal lobe resection combined with orbital undercutting was carried out, the posterior limit of removal being slightly less than 5 cm from the bisected tips of the temporal lobes. After operation the patient gradually became quieter and more cooperative and on September 29, 1952, she was discharged to her home. There have been no neurological sequelae.Psychological Examination. This was performed in April, 1955. Examination revealed a hyperactive woman, too excited and talkative for prolonged testing. She showed a restricted span of attention but scores on verbal intelligence tests were within the dull normal range. Moreover, she appeared to recall some recent happenings quite well. Thus, she knew that her daughter had caught a 7 o'clock train to New York City that morning to buy a dress for a wedding the following Saturday. She could also describe the clothes worn by the secretary who had shown her into the office. However, on formal testing some impairment of recent memory was seen, although unlike the other patients in this group she did succeed on some of the difficult items of the “associate learning” test. As with A. Z. and M. R., the deficit appeared most clearly on tests of delayed recall after a brief interval filled with some other activity. Thus, on the “logical memory” test she gave an adequate version of each story immediately after hearing it, but passing from one story to the next caused her to forget the first almost completely; similar results were obtained for the recall of drawings. We conclude that this patient has a memory impairment identical in type to that of the other patients in this group, but somewhat milder. It is interesting that she had a relatively small excision.Case 7, C. G. This 44-year-old schizophrenic woman had been in the hospital for 20 years without showing any improvement in her psychosis. On November 19, 1951, bilateral medial temporal lobe resection was carried out under local anesthesia, the posterior limit of removal being 5.5 cm from the tips of the temporal lobes. There was temporary loss of consciousness during the resection but the patient was fully conscious at the end of the procedure and postoperative recovery was uneventful. There has been no neurological deficit. She is considered to be in better contact than before but more forgetful.This patient was examined at Norwich State Hospital in April, 1955, and although she was too distractible for prolonged testing, it was possible to show that she remembered some recent events. For example, she knew that she had been working in the hospital beauty parlor for the past week and that she had been washing towels that morning. Yet formal memory testing revealed the same deficit as that shown by A. Z. and M. R., though less extensive data were obtained in this case.Case 8, A. L. This 31-year-old schizophrenic man had been a patient at Norwich State Hospital since October, 1950. He had first become ill in August, 1950, demonstrating a catatonic type of schizophrenia with auditory and visual hallu

Referência(s)
Altmetric
PlumX