EEG Arousal Norms by Age
2007; American Academy of Sleep Medicine; Volume: 03; Issue: 03 Linguagem: Inglês
10.5664/jcsm.26796
ISSN1550-9397
AutoresMichael H. Bonnet, Donna L. Arand,
Tópico(s)Neuroscience and Music Perception
ResumoFree AccessWomenEEG Arousal Norms by Age Michael H. Bonnet, PhD, Donna L. Arand, PhD Michael H. Bonnet, PhD Address correspondence to: Michael H. Bonnet, Ph.D. (151N), Dayton Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, 4100 W. Third Street, Dayton, OH 45428(937) 267-3910(937) 267-5317 E-mail Address: [email protected] Dayton Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Wright State University, and Wallace Kettering Neuroscience Institute, Dayton, OH Search for more papers by this author , Donna L. Arand, PhD Dayton Department of Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Wright State University, and Wallace Kettering Neuroscience Institute, Dayton, OH Search for more papers by this author Published Online:April 15, 2007https://doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.26796Cited by:70SectionsAbstractPDF ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinkedInRedditEmail ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations AboutABSTRACTStudy Objectives:Brief arousals have been systematically scored during sleep for more than 20 years. Despite significant knowledge concerning the importance of arousals for the sleep process in normal subjects and patients, comprehensive age norms have not been published.Methods:Seventy-six normal subjects (40 men) without sleep apnea or periodic limb movements of sleep, aged 18 to 70 years, slept in the sleep laboratory for 1 or more nights. Sleep and arousal data were scored by the same scorer for the first night (comparable to clinical polysomnograms) and summarized by age decade.Results:There were no statistically significant differences for sex or interaction of sex by age (p > .5 for both). The mean arousal index increased as a function of age. Newman-Keuls comparisons (.05) showed arousal index in the 18- to 20-year and 21- to 30-year age groups to be significantly less than the arousal index in the other 4 age groups. Arousal index in the 31-to 40-year and 41-to 50-year groups was significantly less than the arousal index in the older groups. The arousal index was significantly negatively correlated with total sleep time and all sleep stages (positive correlation with stage 1 and wake).Conclusions:Brief arousals are an integral component of the sleep process. They increase with other electroencephalographic markers as a function of age. They are highly correlated with traditional sleep-stage amounts and are related to major demographic variables. Age-related norms may make identification of pathologic arousal easier.Citations:Bonnet M; Arand D. EEG Arousal Norms by Age. J Clin Sleep Med 2007;3(3):271–274INTRODUCTIONDespite the fact that brief arousals have been scored as a part of evaluation of both research and clinical sleep studies for more than 20 years, comprehensive norms for arousals as a function of age or pathology have not been published. Brief arousal counts are currently published in most sleep research studies and tabulated in most clinical patient reports. The American Sleep Disorders Association published guidelines for the scoring of arousals in sleep tests in 1992.1 A few studies have reported arousal index (AI) based upon the American Sleep Disorders Association criteria in various age groups. Wong et al2 reported an AI of 7.6 (SEM 1.11) in normal 6-year-old children. Mathur and Douglas3 reported a significant (r = .6) correlation between age and AI in a population of 55 normal subjects with an age range of 20 to 70 years. Gosselin et al4 reported AI in groups of young (20–35 years) and older (50–65 years) normal sleepers. They did not find a significant difference as a function of age. The AI was 10.3 (SEM 1.0) in the younger group and 11.7 (SEM 1.12) in the older group. Finally, Boselli et al5 reported arousal data from a group of 40 subjects with an age range of 10 to 80 years and showed a high correlation between AI and age (r = .85). However, parametric data were only presented from broad age bands. The American Academy of Sleep Medicine has recently produced an evidence-based review of arousals and arousal scoring that summarizes scoring reliability data and provides empiric support for the importance of scoring arousals, as defined in 19926 without normative data. Although 2 of 3 studies have shown age effects, useful decade-specific norms are currently not available for arousals. This paper provides those norms by decade.Data from a number of previous studies of normal sleepers from a broad age range7–14 were rescored by a single trained scorer with careful attention to the American Sleep Disorders Association guidelines and reliability to produce sufficient data for each decade of adult life through the sixth decade.METHODSubjectsSubjects were selected from previous studies of normal sleepers with the goal of identifying at least 12 subjects in each decade of life from teenaged through 60-year-old subjects. Recruitment and general requirements for participation in research by normal individuals have been consistent over the time period examined. All subjects had originally been selected from ads in the local papers (including local university papers) for participants in sleep research. Individuals considered further had completed a screening questionnaire that indicated that they did not consume more than 250 mg of caffeine daily. Selected subjects denied problems with their sleep. Specifically, they reported that their sleep latency was less than 30 minutes and that they were not bothered by frequent awakenings or early morning awakening. They reported that they usually did not take naps on weekdays. They reported that their usual time in bed on weekdays was between 7 and 9 hours. Potential subjects who had histories strongly suggestive of circadian desynchrony (eg, shift workers), sleep apnea, or periodic leg movements were excluded. Subjects with a history of psychiatric care or use of psychoactive medication were excluded.All subjects were assigned their own bedroom with a desk and chair. Subjects participated in the study in groups of 1 to 2 individuals. The bedroom windows were blocked to eliminate daylight, and subjects performed all study procedures alone in their assigned room. Time cues were not available in the subject rooms, but no attempt was made to limit access to time cues in the other areas of the lab.Sleep recordings (LE-A2, RE-A2, C3-A2, OZ-A1, V5-right clavicle, airflow, chest movements and leg electromyogram) were made during night. Subjects found to have sleep apnea or periodic leg movements were excluded from the study. All sleep recordings were scored in 30-second epochs using Rechtschaffen and Kales15 criteria. The electroencephalographic (EEG) montage was chosen to maximize identification of alpha, as recommended by the American Sleep Disorders Association.1DesignThe initial night in the sleep laboratory was selected for analysis. This night was chosen both because it was most available and because it corresponds to the night that is most commonly analyzed in patient evaluations. As a result, data should be comparable with those from most clinical polysomnograms. However, it has also been shown that a significant first-night effect does not exist for arousals16 so that the data should also be comparable with data from other laboratory nights. Subjects arrived at the laboratory 1 to 2 hours prior to their normal bed time to allow for placement of recording devices. Subjects had an undisturbed night of sleep and were allowed to leave in the morning after recording apparatus had been removed.ScoringA single trained scorer achieved 90% sleep-stage and arousal scoring reliability with laboratory gold-standard recordings. The scorer then rescored all of the records for sleep stages, EEG arousals, respiration, and limb movements to ensure consistent and contemporaneous scoring using Rechtschaffen and Kales15 criteria and American Sleep Disorders Association arousal criteria.1 A laboratory gold-standard recording was scored after each 10 records to demonstrate continuing reliability against lab standards. Recordings from the data set were also rescored periodically. The median arousal scoring reliability from 7 observations was 89%.ResultsDemographic data for the 76 subjects by age group are presented in Table 1. Sleep-stage and arousal data are presented by age group in Table 2. It can be seen that there was a significant increase in both total arousals and AI as a function of age. For AI, the index was similar in 18- and 20-year age groups and significantly lower than in the 30- and 40-year age groups, which were in turn significantly lower than in 50- and 60-year age groups. There were no statistically significant differences for sex or interaction of sex by age (p > .5 for both).Table 1 Demographic DataAge Group, ySubjects, no.Men, no.Weight, lbBMI, kg/m218–20123153 ± 6.323.2 ± 0.5821–30137157 ± 6.123.5 ± 0.8031–40137163 ± 6.125.4 ± 0.8141–50106164 ± 7.325.4 ± 0.8651–601412184 ± 6.127.6 ± 1.3561–701412187 ± 5.927.8 ± 0.61Weight and body mass index (BMI) are expressed as mean ± SEM.Table 2 Mean Total ASDA Arousals, Arousal Index, and Arousals Corrected for Arousals Associated With Limb Movements and ApneaParameterAge Group, yF valueDifference18–2021–3031–4041–5051–6061–70EEG Arousals80 ± 2783 ± 33116 ± 44109 ± 29128 ± 40130 ± 424.43*eAI10.6 ± 4.010.8 ± 4.616.8 ± 6.216.5 ± 5.621.9 ± 8.921.9 ± 6.88.15*aTST, min43944640339535835013.4*aSleep stage, % of TST179121416163.98*b25254525045462.35*gSWS1913773311.6*cREM1819181417151.76Awakenings, no.21.522.929.834.743.3426.88*fTIB, min4804804804804654562.48*None differSE, %95.194.688.085.379.280.716.4*aREM latency10812511410384.51141.13Leg Index4377420.68Leg AI0.80.81.41.80.41.61.69Apnea Index0000.71.31.80.76Apnea AI0000.30.50.2NSFinal AI9.810.116.014.916.221.26.06*dASDA refers to American Sleep Disorders Association; EEG, electroencephalogram; AI, arousal index; TST, total sleep time; SWS, slow-wave sleep; TIB, time in bed; SE, sleep efficiency; REM, rapid eye movement sleep;a: 18 = 20 < 30 = 40 < 50 = 60;b: 18 < 40 = 50 = 60; 20 < 60;c: 18 > 20 > 30 = 40 = 50 = 60d: 18 = 20 < 30 = 40 = 50 < 60;e: 18 = 20 < 50 = 60;f: 18 = 20 = 30 < 50 = 60g: 20 > 50Expected differences were found in sleep-stage amounts as a function of age. There were significant decreases in total sleep time, slow-wave sleep, and sleep efficiency, with significant increases in stage 1 sleep and awakenings.The subject population was controlled to eliminate patients with sleep apnea or periodic limb movements. However, apneas and limb movements were found in all age groups. These are also summarized in Table 2, along with arousals associated with these events so that the level of arousals associated with these events could be calculated as a function of age. Significant age-related effects were not found for any apnea or limb-movement variable. However, a final AI, with arousals associated with apnea and limb movements subtracted from the AI was also calculated. These “corrected” data are still consistent with a general increase in arousals as a function of age.Because similar changes in arousals and sleep variables were found as a function of age, AI was correlated with a set of sleep and demographic variables. These data are presented in Table 3. Significant correlations are marked in the table. A significance level of p < .01 was chosen to control for the possibility of chance findings related to the large number of correlations. It can be seen that AI was significantly correlated with all of the sleep variables presented except for rapid eye movement (REM) latency and time in bed. Increased arousals correlated positively with stage 1, wake time, awakenings, and stage changes, as expected from known relationships in disturbed sleep.17 Significant negative correlations were found for total sleep time, stage 2, slow-wave sleep, REM, and sleep efficiency. AI was also significantly and positively correlated with both age and body mass index. The significant correlation between AI and total sleep time remained significant even when age was partialled out (AI × TST.age r = −.47), but the correlation of AI with age became nonsignificant when the effect of total sleep time was eliminated (AI × AGE.tst r = .21). The significant correlation between AI and body mass index disappeared when age was partialled out (AI × BMI.age r = .15).Table 3 Arousal Correlations with Sleep and Demographic VariablesArousal IndexTotal sleep timeStage 1aSWSaREMaTotal sleep time−.67*Stage 1a.50*−.49*Stage 2a−.41*.57*−.54*SWSa−.45*.49*−.55*REMa−.43*.39*−.32*.02Wake.69*−.82*.44*−.53*−.38*Stage changes.39*−.03−.11.37*−.31*Awakenings.64*−.49*.37*−.46*−.30*Time in bed−.24.59*−.22.19.10Sleep efficiency−.71*.89*−.47*.54*.40*REM latency.19.07.10.17−.44*Sexb−.28.41*−.41*.25.07BMI.42*−.40*.22−.36*−.30*Age.58*−.70*.46*−.62*−.20aSleep stage as a percentage of the total sleep time. SWS refers to slow-wave sleep; REM, rapid eye movement sleep; BMI, body mass index.bMen coded as 1 and women as 2.*p < .01DISCUSSIONBrief arousals have been noted as a normal component of sleep for many years.18 As sleep disorders medicine has evolved, the role of arousals in sleep disorders and daytime sleepiness has become apparent.19 However, determination of a pathologic number of arousals in a patient under baseline or treatment conditions has been difficult without knowing the expected numbers of EEG arousals as a function of age.Data from the current study and the other studies reviewed in the introduction are all plotted with standard errors and straight-line regression line in Figure 1. As can be seen from Figure 1, the straight-line fit from the current data is almost a perfect parallel with the Boselli et al5 data shifted lower by 1 arousal per hour. The best-fit line from the current data also intersects with the mean from the Wong et al2 data for 5-year-old children, implying a useful extrapolation to lower age ranges. In agreement with both the Boselli et al5 and Mathur and Douglas3 studies, a significant correlation between age and AI was found in the current data. The correlation was somewhat lower than that reported in the earlier studies, but this could be related to the significantly larger sample size in the current study. The current data do not support the extremely large increase in AI found by Mathur and Douglas3 in older subjects. It is not clear if the Mathur and Douglas3 results represent the relatively small sample size of older subjects, underlying sleep disorders that may not have been well screened, or perhaps the difficulty in scoring arousals in older subjects who have increased high-frequency EEG and increased incidence of alpha intrusion.Figure 1 Arousal index and linear regression line from the current data (AI) and other referenced arousal studies. Standard error for each mean is marked.Download FigureThe strong correlation of AI with other sleep variables validates brief arousals as a normal component of sleep. Increasing arousals with age were very strongly related to increases in wake time and number of awakenings, long recognized as core components of aging sleep.20 The correlation between AI and total sleep time remained statistically significant when the effect of aging was partialled out, but the correlation between AI and age became nonsignificant when total sleep time was controlled. This suggests that the relationship between these EEG parameters also extends beyond a simple age relationship. Similarly, the significant correlation between AI and body mass index was lost when age was controlled.Subject groups in this study were relatively small (10–14 subjects). Small groups limit the likelihood of finding age-related differences, but the group size in this study is similar to the group size in the major reference for EEG sleep norms by age.20 However, the small number of women in the older groups coupled with a majority of women in the youngest group (that nonetheless did not differ from the 21- to 30-year-old sample) could explain the inability to find sex-related changes in arousal. Additional research will be required to more clearly delineate sex differences in EEG arousals.Disclosure StatementThis is not an industry supported study. Drs. Bonnet and Arand have indicated no financial conflicts of interest.REFERENCES1 EEG arousals: scoring rules and examples: a preliminary report from the Sleep Disorders Atlas Task Force of the American Sleep Disorders AssociationSleep19921517384, 11032543 CrossrefGoogle Scholar2 Wong TK, Galster P, Lau TS, et al.Reliability of scoring arousals in normal children and children with obstructive sleep apnea syndromeSleep200427113945, 15532208 CrossrefGoogle Scholar3 Mathur R, Douglas NJFrequency of EEG arousals from nocturnal sleep in normal subjectsSleep1995183303, 7676165 CrossrefGoogle Scholar4 Gosselin N, Michaud M, Carrier J, et al.Age difference in heart rate changes associated with micro-arousals in humansClin Neurophysiol20021131517, 12169335 CrossrefGoogle Scholar5 Boselli M, Parrino L, Smerieri A, et al.Effect of age on EEG arousals in normal sleepSleep1998213517, 9646379 Google Scholar6 Bonnet MH, Doghramji K, Roehrs T, et al.The Scoring of arousal in sleep: reliability, validity, and alternativesJ Clin Sleep Med,20073133145, 17557423 LinkGoogle Scholar7 Bonnet MHEffect of 64 hours of sleep deprivation upon sleep in geriatric normals and insomniacsNeurobiol Aging198678996, 3960268 CrossrefGoogle Scholar8 Bonnet MH, Arand DL24-Hour metabolic rate in insomniacs and matched normal sleepersSleep1995185818, 8552929 CrossrefGoogle Scholar9 Bonnet MH, Arand DLMetabolic rate and the restorative function of sleepPhysiol Behav19965977782, 8778866 CrossrefGoogle Scholar10 Bonnet MH, Arand DLHeart rate variability: sleep stage, time of night, and arousal effectsElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol19971023906, 9191582 CrossrefGoogle Scholar11 Bonnet MH, Arand DLLevel of arousal and the ability to maintain wakefulnessJ Sleep Res1999824754, 10646164 CrossrefGoogle Scholar12 Bonnet MH, Arand DLArousal components which differentiate the MWT from the MSLTSleep20012444150, 11403529 CrossrefGoogle Scholar13 Bonnet MH, Arand DLSituational insomnia: consistency, predictors, and outcomesSleep200326102936, 14746386 CrossrefGoogle Scholar14 Bonnet MH, Rosa RRSleep and performance in young adults and older insomniacs and normals during acute sleep loss and recoveryBiol Psychol19872515372, 3502753 CrossrefGoogle Scholar15 Rechtschaffen A, Kales AA manual of standardized terminology, techniques, and scoring systems for sleep stages of human subjects1968Washington, DCPublic Health Service, U.S. Government Printing Office Google Scholar16 Bonnet JP, Arand DLDo arousals demonstrate a first night effect?Sleep200629A340 Google Scholar17 Yang Q, Phillips C, Melehan K, et al.Effects of short-term CPAP withdrawal on neurobehavioral performance in patients with obstructive sleep apneaSleep20062954552, 16676788 CrossrefGoogle Scholar18 Halasz P, Kundra O, Rajna P, et al.Micro-arousals during nocturnal sleepActa Physiol Acad Sci Hung197954112, 232612 Google Scholar19 Carskadon MA, Dement WCNocturnal determinants of daytime sleepinessSleep19825Suppl 2S7381, 7156657 CrossrefGoogle Scholar20 Williams L, Karacan I, Hursch CElectroencephalography of Human Sleep: Clinical Applications1974New YorkJohn Wiley & Sons1169 Google Scholar Previous article Next article FiguresReferencesRelatedDetailsCited byExclusion of EEG-based arousals in wake epochs of polysomnography leads to underestimation of the arousal indexWilson D, Tolson J, Churchward T, Melehan K, O’Donoghue F and Ruehland W Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 18, No. 5, (1385-1393), Online publication date: 1-May-2022. Abnormally abrupt transitions from sleep-to-wake in Huntington’s disease sheep (Ovis aries) are revealed by automated analysis of sleep/wake transition dynamicsSchneider W, Vas S, Nicol A, Morton A and Vyazovskiy V PLOS ONE, 10.1371/journal.pone.0251767, Vol. 16, No. 5, (e0251767) Identifying electronic-sport athletes’ sleep-wake cycle characteristicsGomes M, Narciso F, de Mello M and Esteves A Chronobiology International, 10.1080/07420528.2021.1903480, (1-8) Sleep assessment by means of a wrist actigraphy-based algorithm: agreement with polysomnography in an ambulatory study on older adultsRegalia G, Gerboni G, Migliorini M, Lai M, Pham J, Puri N, Pavlova M, Picard R, Sarkis R and Onorati F Chronobiology International, 10.1080/07420528.2020.1835942, Vol. 38, No. 3, (400-414), Online publication date: 4-Mar-2021. Changes in sleep duration, timing, and variability during the COVID-19 pandemic: Large-scale Fitbit data from 6 major US citiesRezaei N and Grandner M Sleep Health, 10.1016/j.sleh.2021.02.008, , Online publication date: 1-Mar-2021. Polysomnographic characteristics and acoustic analysis of catathrenia (nocturnal groaning)Yu M, Wen Y, Xu L, Han F and Gao X Physiological Measurement, 10.1088/1361-6579/abd235, Vol. 41, No. 12, (125012) Sleep modelled as a continuous and dynamic process predicts healthy ageing better than traditional sleep scoringCesari M, Stefani A, Mitterling T, Frauscher B, Schönwald S and Högl B Sleep Medicine, 10.1016/j.sleep.2020.11.033, , Online publication date: 1-Dec-2020. Validation of an Automatic Arousal Detection Algorithm for Whole-Night Sleep EEG RecordingsChylinski D, Rudzik F, Coppieters ‘t Wallant D, Grignard M, Vandeleene N, Van Egroo M, Thiesse L, Solbach S, Maquet P, Phillips C, Vandewalle G, Cajochen C and Muto V Clocks & Sleep, 10.3390/clockssleep2030020, Vol. 2, No. 3, (258-272) Ultradian modulation of cortical arousals during sleep: effects of age and exposure to nighttime transportation noiseRudzik F, Thiesse L, Pieren R, Héritier H, Eze I, Foraster M, Vienneau D, Brink M, Wunderli J, Probst-Hensch N, Röösli M, Fulda S and Cajochen C Sleep, 10.1093/sleep/zsz324, Vol. 43, No. 7, Online publication date: 13-Jul-2020. Prevalence of obstructive sleep apnea in Alzheimer’s disease patientsGaeta A, Benítez I, Jorge C, Torres G, Dakterzada F, Minguez O, Huerto R, Pujol M, Carnes A, Dalmases M, Gibert A, Farré R, de la Torres M, Barbé F and Piñol-Ripoll G Journal of Neurology, 10.1007/s00415-019-09668-4, Vol. 267, No. 4, (1012-1022), Online publication date: 1-Apr-2020. Characterization of cyclic alternating pattern during sleep in older men and women using large population studiesHartmann S, Bruni O, Ferri R, Redline S and Baumert M Sleep, 10.1093/sleep/zsaa016, Sleep stage dynamics in young patients with sleep bruxismKishi A, Haraki S, Toyota R, Shiraishi Y, Kamimura M, Taniike M, Yatani H and Kato T Sleep, 10.1093/sleep/zsz202, Vol. 43, No. 1, Online publication date: 13-Jan-2020. Associations between sleep and verbal memory in subjective cognitive decline: A role for semantic clusteringManousakis J, Nicholas C, Scovelle A, Naismith S and Anderson C Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 10.1016/j.nlm.2019.107086, Vol. 166, , (107086), Online publication date: 1-Dec-2019. Sleep quality of subjects with and without sleep-disordered breathing based on the cyclic alternating pattern rate estimation from single-lead ECGMendonça F, Mostafa S, Morgado-Dias F, Ravelo-García A and Penzel T Physiological Measurement, 10.1088/1361-6579/ab4f08, Vol. 40, No. 10, (105009) Aircraft Noise Effects on Sleep—Results of a Pilot Study Near Philadelphia International AirportBasner M, Witte M and McGuire S International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 10.3390/ijerph16173178, Vol. 16, No. 17, (3178) Sleep Study Interpretation in Obstructive Sleep ApneaPrabhudesai P, Patankar M and Vardhan A International Journal of Head and Neck Surgery, 10.5005/jp-journals-10001-1369, Vol. 10, No. 2, (42-46), Online publication date: 1-Jun-2019. Assessing the exposure-response relationship of sleep disturbance and vibration in field and laboratory settingsPersson Waye K, Smith M, Hussain-Alkhateeb L, Koopman A, Ögren M, Peris E, Waddington D, Woodcock J, Sharp C and Janssen S Environmental Pollution, 10.1016/j.envpol.2018.09.082, Vol. 245, , (558-567), Online publication date: 1-Feb-2019. Troubles du sommeil dans la maladie de Parkinson, et intérêt de la polysomnographieOuahmane Y, Mounach J, Satté A, Zerhouni A, Bourazza A and Ouhabi H NPG Neurologie - Psychiatrie - Gériatrie, 10.1016/j.npg.2018.04.006, Vol. 18, No. 106, (245-255), Online publication date: 1-Aug-2018. Sleep Disturbances in Child and Adolescent Mental Health Disorders: A Review of the Variability of Objective Sleep MarkersBaddam S, Canapari C, van Noordt S and Crowley M Medical Sciences, 10.3390/medsci6020046, Vol. 6, No. 2, (46) Periodic limb movements in tetraplegiaPeters A, van Silfhout L, Graco M, Schembri R, Thijssen D and Berlowitz D The Journal of Spinal Cord Medicine, 10.1080/10790268.2017.1320874, Vol. 41, No. 3, (318-325), Online publication date: 4-May-2018. WHO Environmental Noise Guidelines for the European Region: A Systematic Review on Environmental Noise and Effects on SleepBasner M and McGuire S International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 10.3390/ijerph15030519, Vol. 15, No. 3, (519) Effects of sleep deprivation on component processes of working memory in younger and older adultsPasula E, Brown G, McKenna B, Mellor A, Turner T, Anderson C and Drummond S Sleep, 10.1093/sleep/zsx213, Vol. 41, No. 3, Online publication date: 1-Mar-2018. Interpretation of sleep studies for patients with sleep-disordered breathing: What the anesthesiologist needs to knowInterprétation des études sur le sommeil pour les patients souffrant de troubles respiratoires du sommeil : ce que les anesthésiologistes doivent savoirMeliana V, Chung F, Li C and Singh M Canadian Journal of Anesthesia/Journal canadien d'anesthésie, 10.1007/s12630-017-0988-8, Vol. 65, No. 1, (60-75), Online publication date: 1-Jan-2018. Le SAHOS de l’adolescent : signes d’appel, diagnostic différentielBeydon N Revue d'Orthopédie Dento-Faciale, 10.1051/odf/2017031, Vol. 51, No. 4, (503-509), Online publication date: 1-Oct-2017. Altered sleep patterns in patients with non-functional GHRH receptorOliveira F, Salvatori R, Marcondes J, Macena L, Oliveira-Santos A, Faro A, Campos V, Oliveira C, Costa U and Aguiar-Oliveira M European Journal of Endocrinology, 10.1530/EJE-17-0145, Vol. 177, No. 1, (51-57) Age affects sleep microstructure more than sleep macrostructureSchwarz J, Åkerstedt T, Lindberg E, Gruber G, Fischer H and Theorell-Haglöw J Journal of Sleep Research, 10.1111/jsr.12478, Vol. 26, No. 3, (277-287), Online publication date: 1-Jun-2017. Assessment of Sleep in Patients Receiving Invasive Mechanical Ventilation in a Specialized Weaning UnitHuttmann S, Wilms K, Hamm C, Magnet F, Windisch W and Storre J Lung, 10.1007/s00408-017-9988-2, Vol. 195, No. 3, (361-369), Online publication date: 1-Jun-2017. Physiological effects of railway vibration and noise on sleepSmith M, Croy I, Ögren M, Hammar O, Lindberg E and Persson Waye K The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 10.1121/1.4983302, Vol. 141, No. 5, (3262-3269), Online publication date: 1-May-2017. Increased Evoked Potentials to Arousing Auditory Stimuli during Sleep: Implication for the Understanding of Dream RecallVallat R, Lajnef T, Eichenlaub J, Berthomier C, Jerbi K, Morlet D and Ruby P Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00132, Vol. 11, Relationship between obstructive sleep apnoea syndrome and sleep bruxism: a systematic reviewJokubauskas L and Baltrušaitytė A Journal of Oral Rehabilitation, 10.1111/joor.12468, Vol. 44, No. 2, (144-153), Online publication date: 1-Feb-2017. Are two halves better than one whole? A comparison of the amount and quality of sleep obtained by healthy adult males living on split and consolidated sleep–wake schedulesRoach G, Zhou X, Darwent D, Kosmadopoulos A, Dawson D and Sargent C Accident Analysis & Prevention, 10.1016/j.aap.2015.10.012, Vol. 99, , (428-433), Online publication date: 1-Feb-2017. Eckert D and Butler J Respiratory Physiology Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 10.1016/B978-0-323-24288-2.00016-7, (167-173.e4), . Bliwise D and Scullin M Normal Aging Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine, 10.1016/B978-0-323-24288-2.00003-9, (25-38.e8), . Bonnet M and Arand D Sleep Fragmentation☆ Reference Module in Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Psychology, 10.1016/B978-0-12-809324-5.00897-X, . Du F, Wang S, Wang J, Dai J, Hou F and Li J Analysis of multiscale sign series entropy of the young and middle-aged electroencephalogram signals 2016 9th International Congress on Image and Signal Processing, BioMedical Engineering and Informatics (CISP-BMEI), 10.1109/CISP-BMEI.2016.7852958, 978-1-5090-3710-0, (1530-1534) Has adult sleep duration declined over the last 50+ years?Youngstedt S, Goff E, Reynolds A, Kripke D, Irwin M, Bootzin R, Khan N and Jean-Louis G Sleep Medicine Reviews, 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.08.004, Vol. 28, , (69-85), Online publication date: 1-Aug-2016. Differential changes and interactions of autonomic functioning and sleep architecture before and after 50 years of ageKuo T, Li J, Kuo H, Chern C and Yang C AGE, 10.1007/s11357-015-9863-0, Vol. 38, No. 1, Online publication date: 1-Feb-2016. EEG beta power and heart rate variability describe the association between cortical and autonomic arousals across sleepKuo T, Chen C, Hsu Y and Yang C Autonomic Neuroscience, 10.1016/j.autneu.2015.12.001, Vol. 194, , (32-37), Online publication date: 1-Jan-2016. The relationship between self-reported sleep disturbance and polysomnography in individuals with traumatic brain injuryLu W, Cantor J, Aurora R, Gordon W, Krellman J, Nguyen M, Ashman T, Spielman L and Ambrose A Brain Injury, 10.3109/02699052.2015.1043947, Vol. 29, No. 11, (1342-1350), Online publication date: 19-Sep-2015. Increased Reward-Related Behaviors during Sleep and Wakefulness in Sleepwalking and Idiopathic NightmaresPerogamvros L, Aberg K, Gex-Fabry M, Perrig S, Cloninger C, Schwartz S and Ferri R P
Referência(s)